Across languages and cultures, humans associate high pitches with elevated spatial positions—in the placement of notes on a musical staff, for example, and even in the language: “Low” and “high,” for instance, are used to describe pitch. Cesare Parise, Katharina Knorre, and Marc Ernst at Bielefeld University in Germany now show that our spatial sense of pitch combines two independent contributions. Our head and outer ears act as frequency- and position-dependent filters that strongly affect our ability to localize sound (see the article by Bill Hartmann in Physics Today, November 1999, page 24). Analyzing such filters, known as head-related transfer functions, the researchers found that the filters transmit more high-frequency energy for sounds arriving from higher elevations. More than that, tens of thousands of samples the researchers made of environmental sounds—indoors and outdoors, urban and rural—revealed that higher-frequency sounds reaching our ears tend to originate from elevated...
Skip Nav Destination
Article navigation
1 June 2014
June 01 2014
Citation
Richard J. Fitzgerald; Our spatial sense of pitch. Physics Today 1 June 2014; 67 (6): 22. https://doi.org/10.1063/PT.3.2411
Download citation file:
PERSONAL SUBSCRIPTION
Purchase an annual subscription for $25. A subscription grants you access to all of Physics Today's current and backfile content.
Sign In
You could not be signed in. Please check your credentials and make sure you have an active account and try again.
23
Views
Citing articles via
France’s Oppenheimer
William Sweet
Making qubits from magnetic molecules
Stephen Hill
Learning to see gravitational lenses
Sebastian Fernandez-Mulligan