This 11-year case study describes the acoustic behaviour of a resident Indian Ocean humpback dolphin during commercial swim-with-dolphin activities in Mozambique. Combining data collected using low-cost action cameras with full bandwidth hydrophone recordings, we identified a temporally stable stereotyped whistle contour that met the SIGnature IDentification bout criteria. This whistle was produced with potential information-enhancing features (bi-phonation and subtle variations in frequency modulation). This case study provides evidence for a single, stable, stereotyped call type from a single individual in a mixed species group, contributing to the growing body of evidence for possible signature whistle use in the Sousa genus.

The acoustic repertoire of Sousa spp. comprises acoustic signals common in delphinids, typically grouped into echolocation clicks, burst pulses, and whistles (Herzing, 2014; Van Parijs , 2000; Yang , 2020). Whistles are frequency-modulated tonal calls strongly associated with social contexts (Tyack, 1997), and humpback dolphins seem to present anatomical adaptations for high frequency communication sounds (Frainer , 2019). Whistle frequency parameters vary across the genus, with Atlantic humpback dolphins (S. teuszii) producing whistles with the lowest fundamental frequency across the genus, with a mean maximum of 8.2 kHz (Weir, 2010). Australian humpback dolphins (S. sahulensis) produce whistles with a higher mean maximum of 10.58 kHz and a maximum recorded fundamental frequency of 22 kHz (although this maximum frequency may have been limited by recording equipment) (Van Parijs and Corkeron, 2001b). The whistle repertoire of Indian Ocean humpback dolphins (Sousa plumbea) has only been reported from India, where mean minimum and maximum frequencies ranged from 7.6–10.2 kHz, respectively, and an absolute maximum frequency of 33 kHz was reported (Bopardikar , 2018).

Indian Ocean humpback dolphins (Sousa plumbea) are listed as endangered on the International Union for the Conservation of Nature Red List (Braulik , 2017), and effective methods to monitor populations at an individual level are needed. Passive acoustic monitoring (PAM) is a well-established research tool for the study of wild cetacean populations through the identification of species-specific calls in long-term acoustic recordings. More recently, the use of PAM for studying delphinids has been extended to individual capture–recapture using “signature whistles” (Erbs , 2017; Fearey , 2022; Longden , 2020; Romeu , 2024). These are individually distinctive calls used to communicate identity information and maintain group cohesion and are well demonstrated in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) (Janik and Sayigh, 2013). Since the 1960s, evidence has supported likely signature whistle use across other odontocete species (Tursiops aduncus) (Gridley , 2014), Stenella longirostris (Rio, 2023), Delphinus delphis (Fearey , 2019), Sousa sahulensis (formally S. chinensis) (Van Parijs and Corkeron, 2001a), Sousa chinensis (Cheng , 2017), Pseudorca crassidens (Rio, 2023), Legenorhynchus obliquidens (Caldwell , 1971), Sotalia guianensis (Lima and Le Pendu, 2014), and Monodon monoceros (Shapiro, 2006). Evidence for signature whistle use has been presented in Sousa spp. under instances of stress, including an injured Indo-Pacific humpback dolphin (S. chinensis) (Cheng , 2017) and from a live stranded Australian humpback dolphin (S. sahulensis, formally described as S. chinensis) (Van Parijs and Corkeron, 2001a). Evidence for signature whistle use has not yet been demonstrated in free-swimming S. plumbea.

To reliably communicate identity information, the acoustic signal should have high inter-individual (i.e., distinctive) and low intra-individual (i.e., stereotyped) variability (Charrier , 2003; Janik , 2006; Sayigh , 1990). This can be achieved through inter-individual differences in the vocal apparatus, resulting in by-product distinctiveness (so-called voice features) (Taylor and Reby, 2010). Provided that these differences remain stable over time and the receiver can perceive these differences, identity information can accurately be encoded and decoded by the signaler and receiver, respectively. Alternatively, and more cognitively complex, is the use of “designed individual signatures,” which can develop through a process of production learning. For bottlenose dolphins, there is strong evidence that signature whistles are developed through a process of vocal production learning (Janik, 2009), where the acoustic environment experienced by the dolphin can influence the overall frequency modulation pattern of the “crystalised” signature whistle (Fripp, 2005; Kershenbaum , 2013). Signature whistles are established in calves as early as the first 2 months of life, with high production rates in mother-calf pairs who use signature whistles to facilitate reunions after short separation periods (Janik, 2009; King , 2016; Smolker , 1993). As cohesion calls, signature whistles are produced at elevated rates during separation or isolation events and high arousal states, including times of stress (Caldwell , 1990; Esch , 2009; Probert , 2021; Probert , 2023). In these contexts, emissions may become less stereotyped and more variable in nature, occasionally with the production of non-linear phenomena (Sportelli , 2022).

Like other species within the genus, the habitat preference of S. plumbea includes estuaries and murky rivers, and they are rarely held in captivity; therefore, underwater observations of humpback dolphins are rare, and the context of sound production is often unknown. It is generally accepted that the acoustic signals produced by dolphins originate from the monkey-lips dorsal bursa complex (Cranford , 1996), and that bi-phonation is possible, whereby dolphins can produce whistles and pulsed calls (clicks and burst pulses) simultaneously (Kriesell , 2014; Madsen , 2013). Unlike other wild dolphin populations, information on individual acoustic behaviour in S. plumbea, including signature whistle production, and bi-phonation, is restricted (Bonato , 2015; Herzing, 1996). However, Ponta do Ouro, Mozambique, is one of the few places where long-term in-water interactions with this species have been recorded. Using historical data, we investigate the whistling behaviour of S. plumbea using a case study of a resident individual in Ponta do Ouro, Mozambique, named Herme, using in-water video and acoustic recordings.

Ponta do Ouro Partial Marine Reserve (26.84°S, 32.88°E) is Mozambique's southernmost conservation area. Boat-based research and dolphin tourism involving swim-with-dolphin experiences have been taking place in Ponta do Ouro since 1994, mainly targeting inshore Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus). Over 250 bottlenose dolphins have been catalogued within this population, of which 60 are considered “resident,” and live sympatrically with S. plumbea (Gullen, 2020a). Comparatively, only a small number of S. plumbea (n = 6) have been identified and catalogued during this time (Gullen, 2020b). One humpback dolphin, locally named Herme, was born in 2006 and is the most commonly sighted S. plumbea in the study area and is exclusively encountered with T. aduncus groups.

Acoustic and video data were opportunistically collected between 2009 and 2019 during commercial swim-with-dolphin activities in the Ponta do Ouro Bay. During 2020, additional acoustic data were collected with a focus on S. plumbea. Acoustic data were collected by permitted tour operators and/or under research permits from an 8 m dive boat with two 90 hp four stroke engines. Encounters were limited to 30 min, and a maximum of two in-water encounters were permitted per launch (up to three launches/day).

Once dolphins were sighted and before they were approached, the group was observed for 5 min, and swimmers did not enter the water if the dolphins showed restless behaviour, if they had newborns in the group, or if they were travelling. If conditions were suitable, dolphins were close (<20 m), and it was considered safe to do so, swimmers entered the water to the side or ahead of the group, according to strict regulations to minimise disturbance. During underwater encounters, audio and visual data were collected in close proximity to both T. aduncus and S. plumbea using the equipment summarised in Table 1.

Table 1.

Equipment and recording summary for video and acoustic data collected on the individual Sousa plumbea, Herme, Ponta do Ouro, from 2009–2020. Data collection included authors Angie Gullen (A.G.), Sasha Dines (S.D.), and Rachel Probert (R.P.).

Dates Recorder Recording equipment Sample rate (kHz) Frequency response Total recording duration (hh:mm:ss) Days (encounters)
2009–2019  A.G.  Sony (Tokyo, Japan) HD FDR F × 7 video camera in an Ikalite (Indianapolis, IN) housing or GoPro (San Mateo, CA) Hero4 video camera  48  15 kHz roll-off  01:29:49  30 
2020  S.D, R.P.  SoundTrap (Ocean Instruments Inc., Auckland, New Zealand) hydrophone ST300 GoPro Hero4 video camera  576 (16 bit, mono)  288 kHz  00:15:55 
Dates Recorder Recording equipment Sample rate (kHz) Frequency response Total recording duration (hh:mm:ss) Days (encounters)
2009–2019  A.G.  Sony (Tokyo, Japan) HD FDR F × 7 video camera in an Ikalite (Indianapolis, IN) housing or GoPro (San Mateo, CA) Hero4 video camera  48  15 kHz roll-off  01:29:49  30 
2020  S.D, R.P.  SoundTrap (Ocean Instruments Inc., Auckland, New Zealand) hydrophone ST300 GoPro Hero4 video camera  576 (16 bit, mono)  288 kHz  00:15:55 

Videos of encounters with Herme present were confirmed by author A.G. through visual confirmation of Herme's identifiable features. The videos were scanned for periods of whistles produced by Herme, whereby the individual was the only humpback dolphin present within the mixed species group, and the dolphin was both audibly and visually (through the production of concurrent blowhole movements and/or bubblestream emissions) calling. The acoustic tracks of the video files from 2009–2019 were extracted as mono.wav files (see Table 1) using the behavioural software BORIS (Behavioural Observation Research Interactive Software, BORIS, University of Torino, Torino, Italy) (Friard and Gamba, 2016). Acoustic data collected in 2020 from the SoundTrap was matched to the concurrent GoPro video using time stamps, and then decimated to 96 kHz to facilitate ease of whistle analysis.

Whistles were manually annotated and visually classified using Raven Pro v 1.6 (Center for Conservation Bioacoustics, Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY). All whistles of a particular frequency modulation pattern or contour (see Janik , 2013) were manually grouped into whistle types (SWT) by two observers (T.G., S.D.). The inter-whistle interval (IWI) of stereotyped contours was measured, and a bout analysis was applied by identifying periods where at least three out of four whistles are produced in a bout with an IWI of 1–10 s [for the SIGnature IDentification (SIGID) method, see Janik , 2013]. This method was applied to investigate whether the temporal production of this SWT was similar to that observed in common bottlenose dolphins (T. truncatus). Due to data limitations (i.e., the duration of continuous recording), SIGID was only applied to calls recorded during research effort in 2020, comprising 15 min and 55 s of continuous recording with dolphins (Table 1). Standard time–frequency parameters of the whistles were measured [start frequency (startF), end frequency (endF), minimum frequency (minF), maximum frequency (maxF), and duration], together with simultaneous burst pulse calls using the selection function in Raven Pro. The number of inflection points were manually counted. For the burst pulse signals, the peak frequency (peakF) was calculated within a constrained bandwidth between 2–5 kHz, as this was where most of the energy was focused and prevented erroneous measurement due to the simultaneous high amplitude whistles, which occupied the frequency bandwidth above 5 kHz.

Across the 31 encounters with Herme over the 11 years, the individual produced a stereotyped whistle in 13 encounters, with over 110 emissions of this whistle documented. This whistle was only recorded in encounters with Herme present.

This stereotyped whistle was commonly produced in bouts and met the criteria of SIGID on seven out of eight bouts within the 2020 encounter (Janik , 2013). The distribution of inter-whistle intervals and log survivorship plot demonstrated a clear pattern in the temporal production of whistles. Across the 21 stereotyped whistles, with a peak at 1–3 s, 65% of whistles in a bout occurred within 0.25–10 s of another whistle of the same type (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.

Log survivorship plot of inter-whistle intervals of Herme's stereotyped whistle within the 2020 dataset from Ponta do Ouro, Mozambique.

Fig. 1.

Log survivorship plot of inter-whistle intervals of Herme's stereotyped whistle within the 2020 dataset from Ponta do Ouro, Mozambique.

Close modal

This stereotyped whistle produced by Herme was higher in minF (5.94 ± 1.00 kHz), lower in mean maxF (14.64 ± 1.28 kHz), and longer in duration (1.7 ± 0.7 s), compared to other whistles recorded from the same encounter, which were most likely emitted by T. aduncus. The contour shape of Herme's whistle type showed a relatively stable frequency modulation pattern over the time span [Fig. 2(A)] and was highly distinctive from the other whistle types, including from T. aduncus in the area [Fig. 2(B)], which in the most part produced SWT, which conform to an upsweep multi-loop pattern (see Probert , 2023). Hermes's whistle was more complex, having multiple inflection points (mean 9.2 ± 4.9, compared to T. aduncus mean 3.6 ± 3.3), and an introductory loop that differed from successive loops, with distinct periods of bi-phonation and other non-linear phenomena, such as frequency jumps [see Fig. 2(A)].

Fig. 2.

(A) Long-term stability in the stereotyped whistle type produced by Herme from 2009–2020. Spectrograms shown on axis 0–24 kHz and 0–2 s. Spectrograms also show instances of nonlinear phenomenon (bi-phonation and frequency jumps, see arrows). (B) Examples of signature whistle types from sympatric Tursiops aduncus in Ponta do Ouro, showing high intra-specific similarity amongst signature whistle types, SWT (T. aduncus SWT repertoire from Probert , 2023).

Fig. 2.

(A) Long-term stability in the stereotyped whistle type produced by Herme from 2009–2020. Spectrograms shown on axis 0–24 kHz and 0–2 s. Spectrograms also show instances of nonlinear phenomenon (bi-phonation and frequency jumps, see arrows). (B) Examples of signature whistle types from sympatric Tursiops aduncus in Ponta do Ouro, showing high intra-specific similarity amongst signature whistle types, SWT (T. aduncus SWT repertoire from Probert , 2023).

Close modal

Although this whistle was stereotyped, there were variations in time–frequency components over the 11-year time frame [coefficient of variation (CoV): minF = 17, maxF = 7, dur = 42, Frange = 16, inflection points = 53). The introductory loop was most consistently produced, with greater variability in the occurrence, duration, and number of inflection points of the subsequent tail of the whistle contour [Fig. 2(A)]. The whistle was mostly emitted as a continuous contour, with a maximum duration of 3.6 s. However, a disconnected version of the whistle, including periods of silence, was produced on three occasions.

Non-linear phenomena, such as frequency jumps and biphonation, were also present in Herme's whistle type (63/110 whistles, 57%). Frequency jumps were present in 31 whistle emissions. One to seven discrete burst pulse sounds were recorded during 43% (47/110) of Herme's whistle emissions, providing evidence of biphonation. These burst pulses were highly stereotyped in terms of duration (0.2 ± 0.07 s, CoV = 33) and peakF (3.43 ± 0.43 kHz, CoV = 13). On only one occasion was a burst pulse (with the same characteristics) produced immediately preceding the whistle type. In all other emissions, the burst pulses were produced at intervals separated by 0.1–0.2 s throughout the whistle duration.

This case study provides insight into the acoustic behaviour of a single Sousa plumbea individual encountered exclusively in a free-swimming mixed species group with Tursiops aduncus over an 11-year period. The production of a single stereotyped call type in bouts by Herme shows evidence of both individual distinctiveness and temporal stability. In addition, the whistle type presents evidence of non-linear phenomena and variability in whistling behaviour possibly linked to arousal.

Signature whistle types (SWTs) are well-documented in bottlenose dolphins (Caldwell , 1990; Gridley , 2014; Janik and Sayigh, 2013) and represent the best example of an individual acoustic label formed through vocal learning (Sayigh , 2007). Evidence for signature whistle use has been documented in other Sousa species through opportunistic recordings of isolated or live stranded individuals (Cheng , 2017; Van Parijs and Corkeron, 2001a) Sousa are therefore often included as a genus in which signature whistles are used, based on this limited body of evidence (Fearey , 2019; Janik, 2009). The formation of signature whistles through vocal learning allows the individual to modify the acoustic structure of the sound based on a model sound, crystalising these SWTs over time, but can also be attributed to high levels of interspecific similarity between whistles in mixed species groups (Favaro , 2016). In this case study, the degree of dissimilarity of Herme's whistle to sympatric Tursiops aduncus indicates a lack of whistle convergence occurring between the two species. As well as this, the bout production of Hermes stereotyped whistle fit SIGID criteria, indicating these whistles follow expected signature whistle temporal production patterns. Although this whistle type produced by Herme cannot be classed as a signature whistle based on this limited dataset, this case study contributes to a growing body of evidence that signature whistles are produced within the Sousa genus.

The stereotyped whistle produced by Herme varied in duration and loop structure, which could be indicative of the recording context. In high arousal contexts, where signalling becomes strained or tremulous, signal production (Esch , 2009; Probert , 2021) and variability in call structure (Briefer, 2012) can increase. The combination of human activity (boat presence, engine noise, swimmers in the water) and mixed species assemblage will have likely increased arousal of the dolphins during the recording context (Scarpaci , 2000). Australian bottlenose dolphins (T. australis) are known to change their behavioural pattern after being exposed to “swim-with-dolphin” tourism occasions (Filby , 2017) and even after the interactions, behaviour patterns did not return to the same levels they were before the interaction (Peters , 2013). Mixed species assemblages can elicit behavioural modifications, for example, Guyana dolphins (Sotalia guianensis) change the frequency of their whistles when interacting with bottlenose dolphins (T. truncatus) at the southern Caribbean coast of Costa Rica (May-Collado, 2010), although these changes are linked to harassment behaviour from bottlenose dolphins. Aggressive behaviour between Herme and T. aduncus individuals was not observed in this study, although he was often observed on the fringes of the social group and rarely involved in social behaviour with T. aduncus (Fig. 3). S. plumbea in Algoa Bay, South Africa, and Zanzibar Island, Tanzania, tend to avoid bottlenose dolphins, potentially to minimise costly interactions (Koper and Plön, 2016; Stensland , 2003). Thus, the variations observed in Herme's whistle type could be related to behavioural changes due to unknown, potentially complex interspecific interactions or to the nature of data collection (i.e., during swim-with-dolphin activities).

Fig. 3.

Still images from video footage during 2020 data collection to show the social organization within these mixed species encounters: Herme (Sousa plumbea) circled in a mixed species group with Tursiops aduncus. Example of the social organization of the mixed species group with Sousa plumbea in the company of Tursiops aduncus but not directly engaged in social activity.

Fig. 3.

Still images from video footage during 2020 data collection to show the social organization within these mixed species encounters: Herme (Sousa plumbea) circled in a mixed species group with Tursiops aduncus. Example of the social organization of the mixed species group with Sousa plumbea in the company of Tursiops aduncus but not directly engaged in social activity.

Close modal

Biphonation of whistles and burst pulses were prevalent in Herme's recorded whistles indicating that the stereotypic use of biphonation might be a purposeful addition of information encoding (Papale , 2015; Sportelli , 2022). Biphonation can be categorized as the production of two fundamental frequencies, harmonically unrelated to each other, from a single sound source (Kriesell , 2014; Papale , 2015), or the production of two call types using different vocal structures, which is often a frequency modulated whistle with an overlapping burst pulse (Lilly and Miller, 1961). In other species, the production of two fundamental frequencies has been suggested to increase the probability of caller identification (Filatova , 2009; Filatova , 2012; Volodina , 2006). These can be used as markers of a pod or a matriline association (Filatova , 2009) or vocal cues for mate–mate and/or parent–chick recognition (Aubin , 2000; Jouventin and Aubin, 2002; Lengagne , 2001; Volodina , 2006). As burst pulses are more directional than whistles, the addition of these to stereotyped whistles might help to strengthen individual identification through directionality and amplification. Alternatively, this could be a result of simultaneous navigation and communication (multitasking) (Sportelli , 2022).

Opportunistically collected acoustic data using low-cost action cameras has previously been validated (Chapuis , 2021; Probert , 2023) Although most of the extracted acoustic data had a maximum usable frequency of 15 kHz, the proximity of the animals to the videographer combined with visual cues of whistle production makes this a useful dataset. Although the use of visual cues, particularly bubblestream emissions, to identify call producer has been critiqued (see Fripp, 2005), this method has previously been used to identify call producers in other dolphin species (Miles and Herzing, 2003; Slack, 2018; van der Woude, 2009), including long-term signature whistle stability in T. aduncus in the same region (Probert , 2023). Furthermore, Probert (2023) suggest that bubblestream production is an unbiased cue for identifying both signature and non-signature whistles in T. aduncus.

In conclusion, this case study documents a single stereotyped whistle that dominated recordings during encounters with a single free-swimming humpback dolphin over 11 years, whilst in a mixed-species group. Through our interrogation of the acoustic behaviour displayed, this case study could support further evidence that signature whistles are produced by Sousa spp. and provides temporal evidence of stereotyped whistle use in a free-swimming S. plumbea. Although this case study contributes to the growing body of evidence for possible signature whistle use, further data are required to fully confirm this, including single species recordings of S. plumbea, and warrants further investigation.

We would like to thank the Maputo National Park (previously known as the Ponta do Ouro Partial Marine Reserve), the Department of Conservation Areas (ANAC) in Mozambique, Natural History Museum Maputo, and previous volunteers and guests with DolphinEncountours.org. This research was funded by the National Research Foundation Marine and Coastal grant (2019–2021).

The authors have no conflicts to disclose.

All research was passive and observational, and all research activities were within the scope of normal activities carried out by Dolphin Encountours Research Center's memorandum of understanding with the Department of Conservation Areas (ANAC). Acoustic monitoring of these animals was non-invasive and carried out under ethics permits issued by Stellenbosch University, University of KwaZulu-Natal, University of Cape Town, and Ponta do Ouro Partial Marine Reserve.

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

1.
Aubin
,
T.
,
Jouventin
,
P.
, and
Hildebrand
,
C.
(
2000
). “
Penguins use the two-voice system to recognize each other
,”
Proc. R. Soc. London B.
267
(
1448
),
1081
1087
.
2.
Bonato
,
M.
,
Papale
,
E.
,
Pingitore
,
G.
,
Ricca
,
S.
,
Attoumane
,
A.
,
Ouledi
,
A.
, and
Giacoma
,
C.
(
2015
). “
Whistle characteristics of the spinner dolphin population in the Comoros Archipelago
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
138
(
5
),
3262
3271
.
3.
Bopardikar
,
I.
,
Sutaria
,
D.
,
Sule
,
M.
,
Jog
,
K.
,
Patankar
,
V.
, and
Klinck
,
H.
(
2018
). “
Description and classification of Indian Ocean humpback dolphin (Sousa plumbea) whistles recorded off the Sindhudurg coast of Maharashtra, India
,”
Mar. Mammal Sci.
34
(
3
),
755
776
.
4.
Braulik
,
G. T.
,
Findlay
,
K. P.
,
Cerchio
,
S.
,
Baldwin
,
R. M.
, and
Perrin
,
W. F.
(
2017
).
Sousa plumbea, Indian Ocean Humpback Dolphin.
IUCN Red List Threatened Species 2017
,
8235
(
January
),
e.T82031633A82031644
.
5.
Briefer
,
E. F.
(
2012
). “
Vocal expression of emotions in mammals: Mechanisms of production and evidence
,”
J. Zool.
288
(
1
),
1
20
.
6.
Caldwell
,
M. C.
,
Caldwell
,
D. K.
, and
Hall
,
N. R.
(
1971
). “
An experimental demonstration of the ability of an Atlantic bottlenosed dolphin to discriminate between playbacks of recorded whistles of conspecifics
,” in
Stanford Research Institute Proceedings of the Seventh Annual Conference on the Biology of Sonar Diving Mammals
, Menlo Park, CA (1970), pp.
141
158
.
7.
Caldwell
,
M. C.
,
Caldwell
,
D. K.
, and
Tyack
,
P. L.
(
1990
). “
Review of the signature-whistle hypothesis for the Atlantic bottlenose dolphin
,” in
The Bottlenose Dolphin
, edited by
S.
Leatherwood
and
R. R.
Reeves
(
Academic Press
,
San Diego, CA
) pp.
199
234
.
8.
Chapuis
,
L.
,
Williams
,
B.
,
Gordon
,
T. A. C.
, and
Simpson
,
S. D.
(
2021
). “
Low-cost action cameras offer potential for widespread acoustic monitoring of marine ecosystems
,”
Ecol. Indic.
129
(
March
),
107957
.
9.
Charrier
,
I.
,
Mathevon
,
N.
, and
Jouventin
,
P.
(
2003
). “
Vocal signature recognition of mothers by fur seal pups
,”
Anim. Behav.
65
(
3
),
543
550
.
10.
Cheng
,
Z.
,
Wang
,
D.
,
Wu
,
H.
,
Huang
,
S.-l.
,
Pine
,
M. K.
,
Peng
,
C.
, and
Wang
,
K.
(
2017
). “
Stereotyped whistles may be first evidence to suggest the possibility of signature whistles in an injured Indo-Pacific humpback dolphin (Sousa chinensis)
,”
Aquat. Mamm.
43
(
2
),
185
192
.
11.
Cranford
,
T. W.
,
Amundin
,
M.
, and
Norris
,
K. S.
(
1996
). “
Functional morphology and homology in the odontocete nasal complex: Implications for sound generation
,”
J. Morphol.
228
(
3
),
223
285
.
12.
Erbs
,
F.
,
Elwen
,
S. H.
, and
Gridley
,
T.
(
2017
). “
Automatic classification of whistles from coastal dolphins of the southern African subregion
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
141
(
4
),
2489
2500
.
13.
Esch
,
H. C.
,
Sayigh
,
L. S.
,
Blum
,
J. E.
, and
Wells
,
R. S.
(
2009
). “
Whistles as potential indicators of stress in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
,”
J. Mammal.
90
(
3
),
638
650
.
14.
Favaro
,
L.
,
Neves
,
S.
,
Furlati
,
S.
,
Pessani
,
D.
,
Martin
,
V.
, and
Janik
,
V. M.
(
2016
). “
Evidence suggests vocal production learning in a cross-fostered Risso's dolphin (Grampus griseus)
,”
Anim. Cogn.
19
(
4
),
847
853
.
15.
Fearey
,
J.
,
Elwen
,
S. H.
,
Distiller
,
G.
, and
Gridley
,
T.
(
2022
). “
Improving detectability of dolphin signature whistles for capture-recapture analysis: An examination of array configuration using real-world data
,”
Mar. Mammal Sci.
38
(
4
),
1489
1507
.
16.
Fearey
,
J.
,
Elwen
,
S. H.
,
James
,
B. S.
, and
Gridley
,
T.
(
2019
). “
Identification of potential signature whistles from free-ranging common dolphins (Delphinus delphis) in South Africa
,”
Anim. Cogn.
22
(
5
),
777
789
.
17.
Filatova
,
O. A.
,
Deecke
,
V. B.
,
Ford
,
J. K. B.
,
Matkin
,
C. O.
,
Barrett-Lennard
,
L. G.
,
Guzeev
,
M. A.
,
Burdin
,
A. M.
, and
Hoyt
,
E.
(
2012
). “
Call diversity in the North Pacific killer whale populations: Implications for dialect evolution and population history
,”
Anim. Behav.
83
(
3
),
595
603
.
18.
Filatova
,
O. A.
,
Fedutin
,
I. D.
,
Nagaylik
,
M. M.
,
Burdin
,
A. M.
, and
Hoyt
,
E.
(
2009
). “
Usage of monophonic and biphonic calls by free-ranging resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in Kamchatka, Russian Far East
,”
Acta Ethol.
12
(
1
),
37
44
.
19.
Filby
,
N. E.
,
Christiansen
,
F.
,
Scarpaci
,
C.
, and
Stockin
,
K. A.
(
2017
). “
Effects of swim-with-dolphin tourism on the behaviour of a threatened species, the Burrunan dolphin Tursiops australis
,”
Endang. Species Res.
32
,
479
490
.
20.
Frainer
,
G.
,
Plön
,
S.
,
Serpa
,
N. B.
,
Moreno
,
I. B.
, and
Huggenberger
,
S.
(
2019
). “
Sound generating structures of the humpback dolphin Sousa plumbea (Cuvier, 1829) and the directionality in dolphin sounds
,”
Anat. Rec.
302
(
6
),
849
860
.
21.
Friard
,
O.
, and
Gamba
,
M.
(
2016
). “
BORIS: A free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations
,”
Methods Ecol. Evol.
7
(
11
),
1325
1330
.
22.
Fripp
,
D.
(
2005
). “
Bubblestream whistles are not representative of a bottlenose dolphin's vocal repertoire
,”
Mar. Mammal Sci.
21
(
1
),
29
44
.
23.
Gridley
,
T.
,
Cockcroft
,
V. G.
,
Hawkins
,
E. R.
,
Blewitt
,
M. L.
,
Morisaka
,
T.
, and
Janik
,
V. M.
(
2014
). “
Signature whistles in free-ranging populations of Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops aduncus
,”
Mar. Mammal Sci.
30
(
2
),
512
527
.
24.
Gullen
,
A.
(
2020a
). (personal communication).
25.
Gullen
,
A.
(
2020b
). (personal communication).
26.
Herzing
,
D.
(
1996
). “
Vocalizations and associated underwater behavior of free-ranging Atlantic spotted dolphins, Stenella frontalis and bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus
,”
Aquat. Mamm.
22
(
2
),
61
79
.
27.
Herzing
,
D. L.
(
2014
). “
Clicks, whistles and pulses: Passive and active signal use in dolphin communication
,”
Acta Astronaut.
105
(
2
),
534
537
.
28.
Janik
,
V. M.
(
2009
). “
Acoustic communication in delphinids
,” in
Advances in the Study of Behavior
, 1st ed. (
Elsevier Inc
., Amsterdam), Vol.
40
, pp.
123
157
.
29.
Janik
,
V. M.
,
King
,
S. L.
,
Sayigh
,
L. S.
, and
Wells
,
R. S.
(
2013
). “
Identifying signature whistles from recordings of groups of unrestrained bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
,”
Mar. Mammal Sci.
29
(
1
),
109
122
.
30.
Janik
,
V. M.
, and
Sayigh
,
L. S.
(
2013
). “
Communication in bottlenose dolphins: 50 years of signature whistle research
,”
J. Comp. Physiol. A
199
(
6
),
479
489
.
31.
Janik
,
V. M.
,
Sayigh
,
L. S.
, and
Wells
,
R. S.
(
2006
). “
Signature whistle shape conveys identity information to bottlenose dolphins
,”
Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
103
(
21
),
8293
8297
.
32.
Jouventin
,
P.
, and
Aubin
,
T.
(
2002
). “
Acoustic systems are adapted to breeding ecologies: Individual recognition in nesting penguins
,”
Anim. Behav.
64
(
5
),
747
757
.
33.
Kershenbaum
,
A.
,
Sayigh
,
L. S.
, and
Janik
,
V. M.
(
2013
). “
The encoding of individual identity in dolphin signature whistles: How much information is needed?
,”
PLoS One
8
(
10
),
e77671
.
34.
King
,
S. L.
,
Guarino
,
E.
,
Keaton
,
L.
,
Erb
,
L.
, and
Jaakkola
,
K.
(
2016
). “
Maternal signature whistle use aids mother-calf reunions in a bottlenose dolphin, Tursiops truncatus
,”
Behav. Process.
126
,
64
70
.
35.
Koper
,
R. P.
, and
Plön
,
S.
(
2016
). “
Interspecific interactions between cetacean species in Algoa Bay, South Africa
,”
Aquat. Mamm.
42
(
4
),
454
461
.
36.
Kriesell
,
H. J.
,
Elwen
,
S. H.
,
Nastasi
,
A.
, and
Gridley
,
T.
(
2014
). “
Identification and characteristics of signature whistles in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) from Namibia
,”
PLoS One
9
(
9
),
e106317
.
37.
Lengagne
,
T.
,
Lauga
,
J.
, and
Aubin
,
T.
(
2001
). “
Intra-syllabic acoustic signatures used by the king penguin in parent-chick recognition: An experimental approach
,”
J. Exp. Biol.
204
(
4
),
663
672
.
38.
Lilly
,
J. C.
, and
Miller
,
A. M.
(
1961
). “
Sounds emitted by the bottlenose dolphin
,”
Science
133
(
3465
),
1689
1693
.
39.
Lima
,
A.
, and
Le Pendu
,
Y.
(
2014
). “
Evidence for signature whistles in Guiana dolphins (Sotalia guianensis) in Ilhéus, northeastern Brazil
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
136
(
6
),
3178
3185
.
40.
Longden
,
E. G.
,
Elwen
,
S. H.
,
McGovern
,
B.
,
James
,
B. S.
,
Embling
,
C. B.
, and
Gridley
,
T.
(
2020
). “
Mark–recapture of individually distinctive calls—a case study with signature whistles of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
,”
J. Mammal.
101
(
5
),
1289
1301
.
41.
Madsen
,
P. T.
,
de Soto
,
N. A.
,
Arranz
,
P.
, and
Johnson
,
M.
(
2013
). “
Echolocation in Blainville's beaked whales (Mesoplodon densirostris)
,”
J. Comp. Physiol. A
199
(
6
),
451
469
.
42.
May-Collado
,
L. J.
(
2010
). “
Changes in whistle structure of two dolphin species during interspecific associations
,”
Ethology
116
(
11
),
1065
1074
.
43.
Miles
,
J. A.
, and
Herzing
,
D. L.
(
2003
). “
Underwater analysis of the behavioural development of free-ranging Atlantic spotted dolphin (Stenella frontalis) calves
,”
Aquat. Mamm.
29
,
363
377
.
44.
Papale
,
E.
,
Buffa
,
G.
,
Filiciotto
,
F.
,
Maccarrone
,
V.
,
Mazzola
,
S.
,
Ceraulo
,
M.
,
Giacoma
,
C.
, and
Buscaino
,
G.
(
2015
). “
Biphonic calls as signature whistles in a free-ranging bottlenose dolphin
,”
Bioacoustics
24
(
3
),
223
231
.
45.
Peters
,
K. J.
,
Parra
,
G. J.
,
Skuza
,
P. P.
, and
Möller
,
L. M.
(
2013
). “
First insights into the effects of swim-with-dolphin tourism on the behavior, response, and group structure of southern Australian bottlenose dolphins
,”
Mar. Mammal Sci.
29
(
4
),
E484
E497
.
46.
Probert
,
R.
,
Bastian
,
A.
,
Elwen
,
S. H.
,
James
,
B. S.
, and
Gridley
,
T.
(
2021
). “
Vocal correlates of arousal in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops spp.) in human care
,”
PLoS One
16
(
9
),
e0250913
.
47.
Probert
,
R.
,
Gullan
,
A.
,
Rocha
,
D.
,
Dines
,
S.
, and
Gridley
,
T.
(
2023
). “
Evidence of signature whistles produced by Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) in Mozambique
,”
Bioacoustics
32
(
5
),
580
600
.
48.
Rio
,
R.
(
2023
). “
First acoustic evidence of signature whistle production by spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris)
,”
Anim. Cogn.
26
,
1915
1927
.
49.
Romeu
,
B.
,
Daura-Jorge
,
F. G.
,
Hammond
,
P. S.
,
Castilho
,
P. V.
, and
Simões-Lopes
,
P. C.
(
2024
). “
Assessing spatial patterns and density of a dolphin population through signature whistles
,”
Mar. Mammal Sci.
40
(
1
),
222
236
.
50.
Sayigh
,
L. S.
,
Esch
,
H. C.
,
Wells
,
R. S.
, and
Janik
,
V. M.
(
2007
). “
Facts about signature whistles of bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus
,”
Anim. Behav.
74
(
6
),
1631
1642
.
51.
Sayigh
,
L. S.
,
Tyack
,
P. L.
,
Wells
,
R. S.
, and
Scott
,
M. D.
(
1990
). “
Signature whistles of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus: Stability and mother-offspring comparisons
,”
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
26
(
4
),
247
260
.
52.
Scarpaci
,
C.
,
William Bigger
,
S.
,
James Corkeron
,
P.
, and
Nugegoda
,
D.
(
2000
). “
Bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) increase whistling in the presence of ‘swim-with-dolphin’ tour operations
,”
J. Cetacean Res. Manage.
2
(
3
),
183
.
53.
Shapiro
,
A. D.
(
2006
). “
Preliminary evidence for signature vocalizations among free-ranging narwhals (Monodon monoceros)
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
120
(
3
),
1695
1705
.
54.
Slack
,
M.
(
2018
). “
Bubble stream production by belugas (Delphinapterus leucas)
,”
Theses.
29
, https://digital.sandiego.edu/theses/29
55.
Smolker
,
R. A.
,
Mann
,
J.
, and
Smuts
,
B. B.
(
1993
). “
Use of signature whistles during separations and reunions by wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and infants
,”
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
33
(
6
),
393
402
.
56.
Sportelli
,
J. J.
,
Jones
,
B. L.
, and
Ridgway
,
S. H.
(
2022
). “
Non-linear phenomena: A common acoustic feature of bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) signature whistles
,”
Bioacoustics
32
(
00
),
241
220
.
57.
Stensland
,
E.
,
Angerbjörn
,
A.
, and
Berggren
,
P.
(
2003
). “
Mixed species groups in mammals
,”
Mammal Rev.
33
(
3–4
),
205
223
.
58.
Taylor
,
A. M.
, and
Reby
,
D.
(
2010
). “
The contribution of source-filter theory to mammal vocal communication research
,”
J. Zool.
280
(
3
),
221
236
.
59.
Tyack
,
P. L.
(
1997
). “
Development and social functions of signature whistles in bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus
,”
Bioacoustics
8
(
1–2
),
21
46
.
60.
van der Woude
,
S. E.
(
2009
). “
Bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) moan as low in frequency as baleen whales
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
126
(
3
),
1552
1562
.
61.
Van Parijs
,
S. M.
, and
Corkeron
,
P. J.
(
2001a
). “
Evidence for signature whistle production by a Pacific humpback dolphin, Sousa chinensis
,”
Mar. Mammal Sci.
17
(
4
),
944
949
.
62.
Van Parijs
,
S. M.
, and
Corkeron
,
P. J.
(
2001b
). “
Vocalizations and behaviour of pacific humpback dolphins Sousa chinensis
,”
Ethology
107
(
8
),
701
716
.
63.
Van Parijs
,
S. M.
,
Parra
,
G. J.
, and
Corkeron
,
P. J.
(
2000
). “
Sounds produced by Australian Irrawaddy dolphins, Orcaella brevirostris
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
108
(
4
),
1938
1940
.
64.
Volodina
,
E. V.
,
Volodin
,
I. A.
,
Isaeva
,
I. V.
, and
Unck
,
C.
(
2006
). “
Biphonation may function to enhance individual recognition in the dhole, Cuon alpinus
,”
Ethology
112
(
8
),
815
825
.
65.
Weir
,
C. R.
(
2010
). “
The first description of Atlantic humpback dolphin Sousa teuszii whistles, recorded off Angola
,”
Bioacoustics
19
(
3
),
211
224
.
66.
Yang
,
L.
,
Sharpe
,
M.
,
Temple
,
A. J.
,
Jiddawi
,
N.
,
Xu
,
X.
, and
Berggren
,
P.
(
2020
). “
Description and classification of echolocation clicks of Indian Ocean humpback (Sousa plumbea) and Indo-Pacific bottlenose (Tursiops aduncus) dolphins from Menai Bay, Zanzibar, East Africa
,”
PLoS One
15
(
3
),
e0230319
.