One speech sound can be associated with multiple meanings through iconicity, indexicality, and/or systematicity. It was not until recently that this “pluripotentiality” of sound symbolism attracted serious attention, and it remains uninvestigated how pluripotentiality may arise. In the current study, Japanese, Korean, Mandarin, and English speakers rated unfamiliar jewel names on three semantic scales: size, brightness, and hardness. The results showed language-specific and cross-linguistically shared pluripotential sound symbolism. Japanese speakers associated voiced stops with large and dark jewels, whereas Mandarin speakers associated [i] with small and bright jewels. Japanese, Mandarin, and English speakers also associated lip rounding with darkness and softness. These sound-symbolic meanings are unlikely to be obtained through metaphorical or metonymical extension, nor are they reported to colexify. Notably, in a purely semantic network without the mediation of lip rounding, softness can instead be associated with brightness, as illustrated by synesthetic metaphors such as yawaraka-na hizashi /jawaɾakanaçizaɕi/ “a gentle (lit. soft) sunshine” in Japanese. These findings suggest that the semantic networks of sound symbolism may not coincide with those of metaphor or metonymy. The current study summarizes the findings in the form of (phono)semantic maps to facilitate cross-linguistic comparisons of pluripotential sound symbolism.

Sound symbolism is an iconic, indexical, or systematic association between sound and meaning. In many cases of sound symbolism, a single speech sound is associated with more than one meaning. Numerous experiments have revealed that the high front vowel /i/ may evoke a small, bright, sharp, fast, or precise image, and voiced obstruents are more likely than voiceless obstruents to be associated with darkness and roundedness (Lockwood and Dingemanse, 2015). Winter (2019, p. 2) call this semantic property of sound symbolism “pluripotentiality,” arguing that “the same phonological pattern can have multiple distinct meanings, depending on the context” (see also Kawahara and Kumagai, 2021; Kumagai, 2020; Winter , 2021). In the current study, we dig into this issue using a nonword-based experiment in Japanese, Korean, Mandarin, and English. These languages have been in close cultural contact over centuries but are arguably not phylogenetically related to each other. We report on cases in which one speech sound is linked with more than one meaning, presumably via independent motivations.

The organization of this paper is as follows. Section II introduces previous studies on the pluripotentiality of sound symbolism. Section III describes the method of our cross-linguistic experiment. Section IV presents the results, which will be interpreted in terms of the pluripotentiality of sound symbolism. We discuss the sources of these multiple associations with reference to universal and language-specific factors in Sec. V, where we also make a methodological proposal for describing the semantic networks of sound symbolism for cross-linguistic comparison.

Pluripotentiality in sound symbolism has long been recognized (Joseph, 1994; among others) but probably often as a common feature that does not deserve special attention. Most experiments use specific semantic-differential scales, such as size, brightness, hardness, and cuteness, or visual images, such as rounded “bouba” shapes and spiky “kiki” shapes (Ramachandran and Hubbard, 2001). They typically make specific generalizations using these scales and images, such as “high front vowels are associated with smallness and brightness.” How these meanings are related to each other has been an open question.

Winter (2019) focus on two Korean interjections—khu [khɯ] and khya [khja]—both uttered after downing a shot of liquor. They asked American English speakers to choose which of the two auditorily presented interjections is more suitable for different types of liquor. They found that khya is more strongly associated with softness, smoothness, femaleness, sweetness, and pleasantness. Importantly, Winter (2019) note that “multiple cognitive accounts” would be possible for the observed pluripotentiality.

It could be that the khya/khu distinction taps into the same underlying core meaning which would be a highly abstract multisensory one. In this case, different shades of meaning becoming evident in particular contexts. […] Alternatively, it could be that one of the meanings we investigated (such as, for example, the meaning of “softness”) is the primary meaning, and the other meanings are metaphoric extensions, i.e., the concept of softness is mapped onto other domains. (Winter , 2019, pp. 12–13).

The abstract mapping account [Fig. 1(A)], which posits that different meanings are instances of a general meaning that is associated with a sound, reflects the abstract nature of sound-symbolic phenomena. As for the metaphor account [Fig. 1(B)], in which one meaning extends metaphorically to another, Kawahara (2015) experimentally show that the voiceless obstruents /t, k, s/ are associated with angular shapes and inaccessible types of personality. Angularity and inaccessible personality can be related to each other, even without the mediation of obstruents. In fact, a spiky/edgy personality stands for “an inaccessible personality,” and this metaphor is shared by the Japanese expressions tsuntsun shita hito /tsɯntsɯnɕitäçito/ “an edgy person” and toge no aru ii-kata /toɡenoaɾɯiːkätä/ “a stinging way of speaking.”1

FIG. 1.

Possible types of pluripotential sound symbolism. (A) The two meanings, Y and Z, are instances of the general meaning X that is associated with a sound; (B) Z is a metaphorical (or metonymical) extension from Y; (C) Z is a metaphorical (or metonymical) extension from Y, and Y and Z are instances of the general meaning X; and (D) Y and Z are individually mapped to the same sound.

FIG. 1.

Possible types of pluripotential sound symbolism. (A) The two meanings, Y and Z, are instances of the general meaning X that is associated with a sound; (B) Z is a metaphorical (or metonymical) extension from Y; (C) Z is a metaphorical (or metonymical) extension from Y, and Y and Z are instances of the general meaning X; and (D) Y and Z are individually mapped to the same sound.

Close modal

The two cognitive accounts are unlikely to be mutually exclusive. It appears to be more realistic that some cases of sound symbolism instantiate abstract sound–meaning mappings, whereas other cases are semantic extensions from primary sound–meaning associations. It may also be possible that one case of sound symbolism is a specific instance of an abstract sound–meaning association and, at the same time, a metaphorical (or metonymical) extension from another specific sound–meaning association [Fig. 1(C)]. For example, the obstruent–angularity–inaccessibility association might be motivated by a general link between voiceless obstruents and abruptness as well as by the “personality is an object” metaphor. Furthermore, it is logically possible that the different sound-symbolic meanings of a sound can exist on their own without the mediation of abstract mappings or metaphors/metonymies [Fig. 1(D)].2

This study contributes to this discussion using a cross-linguistic experiment. Presenting the same set of nonword stimuli to speakers of four languages, we investigate which sound–meaning associations are more common among these languages and how these and other associations may arise. Specifically, we identify cases of cross-linguistically shared and language-specific sound symbolism without evident horizontal, metaphorical/metonymical relationships [i.e., Fig. 1(A) or 1(D)]. Based on the results, we also discuss how to advance the study of pluripotential sound symbolism, drawing on the “semantic map,” a linguistic-typological framework for language comparison.

The current study adopts the design of the preliminary experiment by Akita and McLean (2021) in Japanese and English. We combine these data with our newly collected Korean and Mandarin data to reach broader generalizations.

The breakdown of the participants in the four languages is given in Table I. All participants are self-reportedly monolinguals who do not use their nonnative languages on a daily basis. The participants were rewarded for their participation with an Amazon Electronic Gift Card (Seattle, WA; 500 JPY, 5 USD), CUpon (5000 KRW), or bonus points in exams. Consent was obtained electronically.

TABLE I.

Breakdown of the participants.

N Age range (yr) Mean age (yr) Place of residence
Japanese  25 (F, 9; M, 16)  14–24  18.88 (standard deviation, SD = 1.86)  Central Japan 
Korean  43 (F, 22; M, 21)  24–65  36.26 (SD = 9.63)  South Korea 
Mandarin  37 (F, 35; M, 2)  21–27  23.19 (SD = 1.37)  Hong Kong 
English  25 (F, 12; M, 13)  18–33  22.80 (SD = 2.87)  Australia 
N Age range (yr) Mean age (yr) Place of residence
Japanese  25 (F, 9; M, 16)  14–24  18.88 (standard deviation, SD = 1.86)  Central Japan 
Korean  43 (F, 22; M, 21)  24–65  36.26 (SD = 9.63)  South Korea 
Mandarin  37 (F, 35; M, 2)  21–27  23.19 (SD = 1.37)  Hong Kong 
English  25 (F, 12; M, 13)  18–33  22.80 (SD = 2.87)  Australia 

As listed in Table II, 30 audio stimuli were created by systematically combining 5 vowels [i, e, u, o, ä] and six stop consonants [b, d, ɡ, p, t, k] in the initially stressed vowel-consonant-vowel (VCV) form. A female phonetician, whose native language is Japanese, pronounced all stimuli, and three Japanese speakers and three English speakers confirmed that the recordings sound as intended.

TABLE II.

Stimulus words. A glottal stop [ʔ] may precede the first vowels.

[i] [e] [u] [o] [ä]
[b]  [ˈibi]  [ˈebe]  [ˈubu]  [ˈobo]  [ˈäbä] 
[d]  [ˈidi]  [ˈede]  [ˈudu]  [ˈodo]  [ˈädä] 
[ɡ]  [ˈiɡi]  [ˈeɡe]  [ˈuɡu]  [ˈoɡo]  [ˈäɡä] 
[p]  [ˈipi]  [ˈepe]  [ˈupu]  [ˈopo]  [ˈäpä] 
[t]  [ˈiti]  [ˈete]  [ˈutu]  [ˈoto]  [ˈätä] 
[k]  [ˈiki]  [ˈeke]  [ˈuku]  [ˈoko]  [ˈäkä] 
[i] [e] [u] [o] [ä]
[b]  [ˈibi]  [ˈebe]  [ˈubu]  [ˈobo]  [ˈäbä] 
[d]  [ˈidi]  [ˈede]  [ˈudu]  [ˈodo]  [ˈädä] 
[ɡ]  [ˈiɡi]  [ˈeɡe]  [ˈuɡu]  [ˈoɡo]  [ˈäɡä] 
[p]  [ˈipi]  [ˈepe]  [ˈupu]  [ˈopo]  [ˈäpä] 
[t]  [ˈiti]  [ˈete]  [ˈutu]  [ˈoto]  [ˈätä] 
[k]  [ˈiki]  [ˈeke]  [ˈuku]  [ˈoko]  [ˈäkä] 

The experiment in all languages was conducted online using Google Forms (Mountain View, CA). We made two semi-randomized versions of the stimulus set. In version 1, the 30 words were randomized, each followed by three 7-point semantic-differential scales: size (0 “small” ↔ 6 “large”), darkness (0 “dark” ↔ 6 “bright”), and hardness (0 “soft” ↔ 6 “hard”). In version 2, the words were presented in the counterbalanced order of version 1, with the three semantic scales given in the reversed order: hardness, brightness, and size. In each language, half of the participants answered version 1, and the other half answered version 2. The first page of the Google Forms provided the following instructions in the language of the participant:

In this experiment, you will listen to 30 novel words. Imagine they are the names of newly discovered jewels. Your task is to rate the size, brightness, and hardness of each jewel solely based on its name. There are no right answers. Please follow your intuition.

We focused on imaginary jewel names because they serve as a natural context for all three of the semantic scales.

The participants were also instructed to wear headphones or earphones, adjust the volume, and not change it during the experiment. They were allowed to listen to each word as many times as they preferred.

According to the previous experimental literature on sound symbolism, it is expected that the nonfront vowels [u, o, ä], lower vowels [e, o, ä], and voiced obstruents [b, d, ɡ] are associated with largeness (Lockwood and Dingemanse, 2015; Shinohara and Kawahara, 2010; Westbury , 2018), and the rounded vowels [u, o] with darkness and softness (Mok , 2019; see also Johansson , 2020a). Studies comparing basic word lists across languages also suggest that the voiceless non-labial obstruents [t, k] may be linked with hardness (Johansson , 2020b; Johansson, 2017). We replicate and refine these observations using the same auditory stimuli across three semantic dimensions and four groups of participants.

The experiment revealed several interesting sound-symbolic patterns in the four languages. We, first, present the descriptive results of the three ratings one by one and then analyze them with statistical models.

Figure 2 shows the distributions of size ratings in terms of the five relevant phonetic features: vowel frontness (front [i, e] vs nonfront [u, o, ä]), height (high [i, u] vs mid [e, o] vs low [ä]), lip rounding (rounded [u, o] vs unrounded [i, e, ä]), consonant voicing (voiced [b, d, ɡ] vs voiceless [p, t, k]), and place of articulation (bilabial [b, p] vs alveolar [d, t] vs velar [ɡ, k]). In accord with the previous findings, in all languages, nonfront, lower, and rounded vowels tended to be associated with larger images. The effect of obstruent voicing on size ratings was clearest in Japanese speakers. Place of articulation had little influence, if any, on size ratings.

The results of the darkness ratings are presented in Fig. 3. The overall results are, again, consistent with the previous findings. In all languages, except Korean, the nonfront vowels tended to be rated darker than the front vowels. The effect of lip rounding was shared across the four languages: the rounded vowels were darker, although this effect was weak in Korean. Voicing, once more, yielded a sharp contrast only in Japanese. The effect of place of articulation was limited, as with the size ratings.

FIG. 2.

Distributions of size ratings averaged over each stimulus (0 small ↔ 6 large). The black dots represent the means.

FIG. 2.

Distributions of size ratings averaged over each stimulus (0 small ↔ 6 large). The black dots represent the means.

Close modal
FIG. 3.

Distributions of darkness ratings averaged over each stimulus (0 bright ↔ 6 dark). The black dots represent the means.

FIG. 3.

Distributions of darkness ratings averaged over each stimulus (0 bright ↔ 6 dark). The black dots represent the means.

Close modal
FIG. 4.

Distributions of hardness ratings averaged over each stimulus (0 soft ↔ 6 hard). The black dots represent the means.

FIG. 4.

Distributions of hardness ratings averaged over each stimulus (0 soft ↔ 6 hard). The black dots represent the means.

Close modal

Figure 4 shows the results of the hardness ratings. As compared with the size and darkness ratings, the results here look less clear and less consistent across languages. Nevertheless, in all languages, the rounded vowels tended to be rated softer than the unrounded vowels. The bilabial stops were also rated softer than the alveolars in all languages. Interestingly, the velar stops were perceived relatively hard by the Japanese and Mandarin speakers but relatively soft by the Korean and English speakers, to whom the alveolar stops sounded harder.

We used the ordinal package version 2022.11.16 (Christensen, 2022) to perform our cumulative link mixed model (CLMM) analysis in R version 4.3.1 (R Core Team, 2023). CLMMs predict ordinal response variables from fixed and random effects. We fit a separate model for each semantic scale in each language without a Bonferroni correction, given that the three rating tasks and four groups of participants constituted separate datasets to examine. Each model included vowel (i.e., [i, e, u, o, ä]) and two consonantal features (i.e., voicing and place of articulation) as fixed effects and participant and stimulus as random effects. We also included an interaction between voicing and place, anticipating different sound-symbolic values between [d, ɡ] and [t, k] (see D'Onofrio, 2014). [ä] and the voiced bilabials (i.e., [ˈäbä]) were set as a reference level. We did not include the three vocalic features (i.e., frontness, height, and rounding) in the models, as in our stimulus set the rounded vowels [u, o] are both nonfront and [ä] is the only low vowel. Using [ä] as a reference allows us to indirectly see the effects of all frontness ([ä] vs [i, e]), height ([ä] vs [i, e, u, o]), and rounding ([ä] vs [u, o]).

Table III presents the statistically reliable effects obtained in the size models in the four languages. The association between the front vowels and smallness was replicated in all languages. On the other hand, only Japanese speakers rated the voiceless stops to be smaller than the voiced stops. The smallness of the velar stops as compared with the alveolar stops was unique to Korean speakers, and the relative smallness of the rounded vowels was unique to Mandarin speakers.

TABLE III.

Reliable effects in CLMMs for size ratings. “Than X” indicates a change in reference level. Cross-linguistically shared sound–meaning associations are highlighted in bold. *, p < 0.05; **, 0.01; ***, 0.001.

Vowels Frontness Height Rounding Other Consonants Voicing Place
Japanese  [i] = smaller (b = −1.19, standard error (SE) = 0.21, z = −5.72***), [e] = smaller (b = −0.54, SE = 0.21, z = −2.62**)        Voiceless = smaller (b = −0.51, SE = 0.23, z = −2.26*)   
Korean  [i] = smaller (b = −0.54, SE = 0.16, z = −3.42***), [e] = smaller (b = −0.60, SE = 0.16, z = −3.82***)          Velar = smaller than alveolar (b = −0.35, SE = 0.17, z = −2.06*) 
Mandarin  [i] = smaller (b = −2.72, SE = 0.19, z = −14.29***), [e] = smaller (b = −1.24, SE = 0.17, z = −7.14***)    [u] = smaller (b = −0.77, SE = 0.18, z = −4.36***), [o] = smaller (b = −0.69, SE = 0.17, z = −4.03***)       
English  [i] = smaller (b = −1.85, SE = 0.24, z = −7.77***), [e] = smaller (b = −0.57, SE = 0.23, z = −2.52*)           
Vowels Frontness Height Rounding Other Consonants Voicing Place
Japanese  [i] = smaller (b = −1.19, standard error (SE) = 0.21, z = −5.72***), [e] = smaller (b = −0.54, SE = 0.21, z = −2.62**)        Voiceless = smaller (b = −0.51, SE = 0.23, z = −2.26*)   
Korean  [i] = smaller (b = −0.54, SE = 0.16, z = −3.42***), [e] = smaller (b = −0.60, SE = 0.16, z = −3.82***)          Velar = smaller than alveolar (b = −0.35, SE = 0.17, z = −2.06*) 
Mandarin  [i] = smaller (b = −2.72, SE = 0.19, z = −14.29***), [e] = smaller (b = −1.24, SE = 0.17, z = −7.14***)    [u] = smaller (b = −0.77, SE = 0.18, z = −4.36***), [o] = smaller (b = −0.69, SE = 0.17, z = −4.03***)       
English  [i] = smaller (b = −1.85, SE = 0.24, z = −7.77***), [e] = smaller (b = −0.57, SE = 0.23, z = −2.52*)           

Table IV presents the reliable effects obtained from the darkness models. The darkness of [u] was shared across all four languages, whereas that of the other rounded vowel [o] was not shared by the Korean speakers. [e] was also rated relatively dark in Japanese, Mandarin, and English. The Japanese speakers used voicing again, and this time, it was for dark images. The Mandarin speakers also rated [i] brighter than [ä].

TABLE IV.

Reliable effects in CLMMs for darkness ratings. Cross-linguistically shared sound–meaning associations are highlighted in bold. *, p < 0.05; **, 0.01; ***, 0.001.

Vowels Frontness Height Rounding Other Consonants Voicing Place
Japanese      [u] = darker (b = 1.33, SE = 0.24, z = 5.47***), [o] = darker (b = 1.69, SE = 0.25, z = 6.87***)  [e] = darker (b = 0.51, SE = 0.24, z = 2.13*)  Voiceless = brighter (b = −1.20, SE = 0.26, z = −4.57***)   
Korean      [u] = darker (b = 0.46, SE = 0.16, z = 2.92**)       
Mandarin  [i] = brighter (b = −0.63, SE = 0.17, z = −3.65***)    [u] = darker (b = 1.87, SE = 0.18, z = 10.38***), [o] = darker (b = 0.94, SE = 0.17, z = 5.48***)  [e] = darker (b = 0.36, SE = 0.17, z = 2.13*)     
English      [u] = darker (b = 1.61, SE = 0.21, z = 7.51***), [o] = darker (b = 1.36, SE = 0.21, z = 6.42***)  [e] = darker (b = 0.48, SE = 0.21, z = 2.30*)     
Vowels Frontness Height Rounding Other Consonants Voicing Place
Japanese      [u] = darker (b = 1.33, SE = 0.24, z = 5.47***), [o] = darker (b = 1.69, SE = 0.25, z = 6.87***)  [e] = darker (b = 0.51, SE = 0.24, z = 2.13*)  Voiceless = brighter (b = −1.20, SE = 0.26, z = −4.57***)   
Korean      [u] = darker (b = 0.46, SE = 0.16, z = 2.92**)       
Mandarin  [i] = brighter (b = −0.63, SE = 0.17, z = −3.65***)    [u] = darker (b = 1.87, SE = 0.18, z = 10.38***), [o] = darker (b = 0.94, SE = 0.17, z = 5.48***)  [e] = darker (b = 0.36, SE = 0.17, z = 2.13*)     
English      [u] = darker (b = 1.61, SE = 0.21, z = 7.51***), [o] = darker (b = 1.36, SE = 0.21, z = 6.42***)  [e] = darker (b = 0.48, SE = 0.21, z = 2.30*)     

Table V is a summary of the hardness models. The softness of the rounded vowels, especially [u], was shared by the Japanese, Mandarin, and English speakers. [e] was also rated relatively soft in Japanese and Mandarin. Place of articulation turned out to have different effects across languages. The Japanese and Mandarin speakers rated the velars harder than the bilabials, whereas the Korean and English speakers rated the alveolars relatively hard. We obtained a reliable interaction between voicing and place in English, indicating that [k] but not [ɡ] was rated relatively hard. The slopes of these effects are relatively low compared to those of the reliable effects in the size and darkness models, suggesting the weak linkage between sound and hardness.

TABLE V.

Reliable effects in CLMMs for hardness ratings. “Than X” indicates a change in reference level. Cross-linguistically shared sound–meaning associations are highlighted in boldface. *, p < 0.05; **, 0.01; ***, 0.001.

Vowels Frontness Height Rounding Other Consonants Voicing Place
Japanese      [u] = softer (b = −1.13, SE = 0.21, z = −5.38***), [o] = softer (b = −0.54, SE = 0.21, z = −2.57*)  [e] = softer (b = −0.43, SE = 0.21, z = −2.10*)    Velar = harder (b = 0.60, SE = 0.22, z = 2.72**) 
Korean            Alveolar = harder than velar (b = 0.41, SE = 0.17, z = 2.41*) 
Mandarin      [u] = softer (b = −0.63, SE = 0.17, z = −3.62***), [o] = softer (b = −0.69, SE = 0.17, z = −4.05***)  [e] = softer (b = −0.65, SE = 0.17, z = −3.82***)    Velar = harder (b = 0.41, SE = 0.18, z = 2.23*) 
English      [u] = softer (b = −0.61, SE = 0.21, z = −2.89**)      Alveolar = harder (b = 0.58, SE = 0.22, z = 2.64**), [k] = harder (b = 0.94, SE = 0.32, z = 2.96** 
Vowels Frontness Height Rounding Other Consonants Voicing Place
Japanese      [u] = softer (b = −1.13, SE = 0.21, z = −5.38***), [o] = softer (b = −0.54, SE = 0.21, z = −2.57*)  [e] = softer (b = −0.43, SE = 0.21, z = −2.10*)    Velar = harder (b = 0.60, SE = 0.22, z = 2.72**) 
Korean            Alveolar = harder than velar (b = 0.41, SE = 0.17, z = 2.41*) 
Mandarin      [u] = softer (b = −0.63, SE = 0.17, z = −3.62***), [o] = softer (b = −0.69, SE = 0.17, z = −4.05***)  [e] = softer (b = −0.65, SE = 0.17, z = −3.82***)    Velar = harder (b = 0.41, SE = 0.18, z = 2.23*) 
English      [u] = softer (b = −0.61, SE = 0.21, z = −2.89**)      Alveolar = harder (b = 0.58, SE = 0.22, z = 2.64**), [k] = harder (b = 0.94, SE = 0.32, z = 2.96** 

The results of the current experiment revealed cross-linguistically shared and language-specific sound-symbolic perceptions. On one hand, speakers of all four languages associated the front vowels [i, e] with small jewels and the rounded vowel [u] with dark jewels. The softness of the rounded vowels was also relatively common across languages. The strongest associations were found in the domain of size, followed by darkness, while the associations with hardness were less strong. This last result may be partly attributed to the fact that we presented the stimuli as the names of jewels, which, as an anonymous reviewer pointed out, are by their nature on the hard side and may not be compatible with the concept of softness.

We also found some language-specific associations. Only the Japanese speakers used voicing effectively in their size and darkness ratings. Moreover, whereas the Japanese and Mandarin speakers linked the velars [ɡ, k] with hardness, the Korean and English speakers linked the alveolars [d, t] with this attribute.

It appears reasonable to assume that many, if not all, sound–meaning associations shared across many languages are clearly and strongly motivated by the sensory resemblance between the sound and meaning (i.e., iconicity; for related discussions, see Dingemanse , 2016; Johansson , 2020b; Saji , 2019; Shih , 2019; Thompson , 2021). For example, the frontness–smallness association is very common arguably because people can feel the iconic correspondence between the oral cavity size and the object's size, or they can instinctively or experientially associate high F2 with small objects via the frequency code, according to which smaller vocalizers (e.g., mice) tend to make higher-pitched sounds than larger vocalizers (e.g., elephants; Ohala, 1994). The association between lip rounding and darkness is not so well researched but could be mediated by a more general crossmodal correspondence between pitch and brightness, in which lower frequency sounds are perceived as darker than high frequency sounds (Marks, 1975). The acoustic correlate of lip rounding is lower formant frequencies, especially F3, which could lead to rounded vowels sounding dark. In the tactile domain, meanwhile, rounded vowels were linked to softness. It is possible that the softness of the lips matches a soft object or their rounded shape evokes a dull, rather than sharp, image that matches a soft surface (see, e.g., Köhler, 1929).3

In contrast, language-specific sound symbolism is more subject to individual languages' lexical patterns. Japanese speakers' sensitivity to obstruent voicing is a clear example of this. Voicing is the most important phonetic feature in the sound-symbolic system of Japanese ideophones, which very often form minimal pairs that only differ in initial voicing (e.g., kirakira /kiɾakiɾa/ “glittering” vs giragira /ɡiɾaɡiɾa/ “glaring”; porori /poɾoɾi/ “a light object dropping” vs borori /boɾoɾi/ “a heavy object dropping”; tonton /tonton/ “knocking” vs dondon /dondon/ “banging”; Hamano, 1998). The current results demonstrate that Japanese speakers use this phonosemantic pattern in their sound-symbolic perception of nonwords. Note that voicing of stop consonants is not phonemically contrastive in Korean and Mandarin, and Korean, instead, has a three-way distinction of stops as in ca-ta /tɕada / “sleep” (plain), cca-ta /tɕ'ada/ “be salty” (tense), and cha-ta /tɕhada/ “kick” (aspirated). These phonological backgrounds may partly explain why speakers of these languages were not sensitive to voicing symbolism. Recall that Korean speakers, instead, attributed smallness to the velar stops and Mandarin speakers attributed smallness to the rounded as well as front vowels.4

Similarly, the Korean speakers' sound-symbolic perception of the rounded vowels can be attributed to the phonological system of this language. Korean ideophones exhibit vowel harmony according to their unique two-tiered vowel system, which consists of dark [i, e, ɯ, ʌ, u] and “light” vowels [ɛ, ø, a, o] (Kim-Renaud, 1978; Sohn, 1999). Dark vowels typically co-occur with dark vowels (e.g., phwungten /phuŋdʌŋ/ “a large object splashing”), and light vowels with light vowels (e.g., phongtang /phoŋdaŋ/ “a small object splashing”), expressing dark/heavy/large and bright/light/small meanings, respectively. Our Korean participants perceived [u] but not [o] as dark precisely because of this language-specific sound-symbolic system.

Language-specific sound symbolism may also arise from the analogy to specific ideophones or other lexical items. The Japanese speakers' association between the velars and hardness may be partly attributed to Japanese words for hard texture, such as katai /katai/ “be hard,” kachikachi /katɕikatɕi/ “stone-hard,” and gachigachi /ɡatɕiɡatɕi/ “hard-frozen” (for a related discussion, see Kumagai , 2022). The Korean speakers' perception of [t] as hard might come from the common texture adjective ttakttak-hata /t'akt'akhada/ “be hard.” The English speakers' associations between [d, t, k] and hardness might also be related to certain onomatopoeic words, such as dingdong, tap, crack, and crunch. Similarly, in Mandarin, the word ying4 /ɪŋ˥˩/ hard contains a velar.

The conflicting results in the English data, which linked [k] with hardness but not [ɡ], might also have a lexical explanation. The English phonestheme kn, in words such as knee, knob, knot, knuckle, and knoll, is associated with round things, which as discussed could, in turn, be related to softness. Crucially, etymologically, these words go back to Proto-Indo-European roots in which the velar stop was actually voiced (e.g., knee goes back to PIE *ĝenu “knee,” knot, knob, and knuckle go back to PIE *gen “to pinch, clench, ball up”). While there is no way to know for certain that this is the source of English speakers' tendency to consider [ɡ], in particular, as relatively soft, previous studies have found evidence for iconic associations in languages having a long history and even persisting long after sound change has obscured the original phonetic motivations for the associations (Carling and Johansson, 2014; Philps, 2023; Winter , 2022).

The current study identified a few cases of pluripotential sound symbolism. Here, we consider how the different meanings are related to each other.

The associations between the rounded vowels and dark and soft images were found most widely. Both sound–meaning associations have relatively clear and strong iconic motivations. As we discussed in Sec. V A, the low frequency of the rounded vowels may evoke a dark image, and the soft texture and rounded shape of the lips may mediate these vowels and softness. Crucially, it is difficult to draw a direct link between darkness and softness. In fact, we do not know any conventional metaphors or metonymies in the four languages in which a word for “dark” means “soft” or a word for “soft” means “dark” (a soft light would not mean “a dark light”). Without the mediation of the rounded vowels, it would even be possible that softness is associated with brightness rather than darkness, presumably, through the positive valence they have in common. For example, the Japanese words yawaraka-na /jawaɾakana/ “be soft” and hizashi /çizaɕi/ “sunshine” but not hikage /çikaɡe/ “shade” have relatively positive meanings, which allow yawaraka-na hizashi to mean “a gentle sunshine” as a so-called synesthetic metaphor (Winter, 2019).5,6

Another example of pluripotentiality is the Japanese speakers' associations between voicing and size and darkness. Both sound–meaning correspondences have an iconic basis. Voiced obstruents in Japanese involve greater energy than their voiceless counterparts and also expand the oral cavity to lower the air pressure (Kawahara , 2018). These phonetic characteristics of voicing may motivate its association with large size. The voicing–darkness link can be attributed to the relatively low frequency of voiced obstruents just as with the rounding–darkness association. Thus, the two sound–meaning associations have their own reasons to exist. The two concepts, size and darkness, are not related to each other in the lexicon either. Big night would not mean “dark night,” and a dark house would not mean “a big house.”

Finally, the Mandarin speakers rated [i] as small and bright. As discussed in Sec. V A, the smallness of [i] is straightforwardly attributed to its articulatory and acoustic properties via iconicity (and perhaps indexicality). The association between [i], a vowel with high F2, and brightness is another example of the pitch–brightness correspondence. It is, again, difficult to find a direct metaphorical or metonymical link between the two concepts. A small room would not mean “a bright room,” and a bright dog would not mean “a small dog.”

These results confirm that one sound can be iconically associated with different meanings without the mediation of metaphor or metonymy.7,8 In fact, none of the above pairs of concepts are reported to “colexify” (i.e., cross-linguistically tend to be encoded in the same lexemes; Rzymski , 2020). However, it remains unclear whether these meanings instantiate an abstract, multisensory image [i.e., Fig. 1(A)] or exist independently [i.e., Fig. 1(D)].9 It is also worth investigating whether iconicity can “block” metaphorical extension in sound symbolism as reported for sign language (Meir, 2010) and ideophones (Akita, 2013; see also Winter, 2019). Furthermore, there is the issue of sound–meaning associations based on lexical analogy (e.g., the alveolar stops for hardness in English, Sec. V A). It remains to be investigated whether the sounds in these analogy-mediated associations are also pluripotential or restricted to specific semantic scales due to their strong relations to particular lexical items. To answer this question, future research will need to test more semantic scales.

We close our discussion with a visual summary of our findings on the pluripotentiality of sound symbolism using what we term “phonosemantic” maps (cf. McLean, 2021; Van Hoey, 2024). Our phonosemantic maps are inspired by semantic maps primarily found in typological investigations of grammatical morphemes, where they are used to track and compare cross-linguistic patterns of polysemy and semantic change. For example, Haspelmath (2003) draws a maximal cognitive space for dative morphemes across languages that covers DIRECTION, RECIPIENT, PURPOSE, BENEFICIARY, etc. Dative morphemes in individual languages, such as English to and French à, are mapped on this space, allowing us to make cross-linguistic generalizations. For example, all dative morphemes that can express BENEFICIARY [e.g., Ai ni keeki o yaku /ainikeːkiojakɯ/ “bake a cake for Ai” (Japanese)] can also express RECIPIENT (e.g., Ai ni keeki o ageru /ainikeːkioaɡeɾɯ/ “give a cake to Ai”) but not vice versa.

In a typical semantic map, the base of the map is the cognitive space, and it is onto this cognitive space that the functions of different grammatical morphemes are mapped. In our phonosemantic maps, on the other hand, the base of the map is, instead, the phonological (or phonetic) space, and it is onto this phonological space that phonosemantic associations are mapped. Whereas in a typical semantic map, the assumption is that semantic extension occurs primarily through conceptual links, in our phonosemantic maps, we assume that semantic extension occurs primarily via links mediated by sound symbolism. That is, it is the inherent properties of the sounds themselves that provide the natural links between concepts, not necessarily the semantic properties of the concepts (as in metaphor and metonymy). The phonosemantic maps for the results of our experiment are shown in Fig. 5, which is, again, based on the CLMMs whose reference levels are [ˈäbä] in most cases.

FIG. 5.

(Color online) Partial phonosemantic maps for Japanese, Korean, Mandarin, and English speakers' sound-symbolic associations. Labeled boxes indicate which sounds different languages have a semantic association with, e.g., the blue box labeled DARK around [e], [u], and [o] indicates that English has a phonosemantic association between these vowels and the concept DARK.

FIG. 5.

(Color online) Partial phonosemantic maps for Japanese, Korean, Mandarin, and English speakers' sound-symbolic associations. Labeled boxes indicate which sounds different languages have a semantic association with, e.g., the blue box labeled DARK around [e], [u], and [o] indicates that English has a phonosemantic association between these vowels and the concept DARK.

Close modal

The phonosemantic map representation effectively shows how each sound behaves sound symbolically and how the behavior is shared across languages. For example, Fig. 5 suggests that the relationship between the three semantic scales is not uniform: the relationship between size and hardness might be weaker than the other two relationships. We will be able to test this hypothesis by increasing the amount of data (see Westbury , 2018, for a related discussion).

Studies on sound symbolism have accumulated experimental and descriptive evidence for numerous sound–meaning associations in numerous languages. Phonosemantic maps will serve as a cross-linguistic common ground for sound symbolism researchers to put the evidence together and draw general conclusions.

In this paper, we tested the sound-symbolic intuitions of speakers of four languages from different phyla on three semantic scales: size, brightness, and hardness. A few phonetic features were iconically associated with two scales: lip rounding for darkness and softness in Japanese, Mandarin, and English, Japanese voicing for large size and darkness, and Mandarin [i] for smallness and brightness. We analyzed these cases as pluripotential sound symbolism without metaphorical or metonymical mediation. In particular, darkness and softness symbolisms suggest that the semantic networks of sound symbolism can differ from those of (synesthetic) metaphor and metonymy. We summarized our findings with a phonosemantic map approach to the description of sound symbolism that is expected to facilitate further investigations into pluripotential sound symbolism that cover a wider range of languages and cultures (Ćwiek , 2021). We hope that future research will enrich the maps to illuminate the role of abstraction and metaphor/metonymy in sound symbolism.

Our sincere gratitude goes to Susanne Fuchs and the two anonymous reviewers of The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America (JASA), whose comments helped us significantly improve this paper. We also thank John L. A. Huisman for his advice in statistical analysis. This work was supported by Japanese Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (C; Grant No. 20K00567).

The authors have no conflicts to disclose.

The experiment reported in this paper was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the Australian National University (Project No. 2018/544). All participants gave online informed consent and agreed on the publication of their anonymized data.

The data that support the findings of this study, all experimental stimuli, data, and R code are available through the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/3vdre/.

1

Metonymy may also link different meanings in sound symbolism. Kawahara (2018) present a related example in which the names of larger and heavier Pokémons tend to have more voiced obstruents. Size and weight are associated with each other via metonymy (i.e., large objects tend to be heavy) rather than metaphor.

2

Kantartzis (2011) pursues this issue in terms of the opposition of “domain-general” vs “domain-specific.” She suggests, in favor of the domain-general view, that “shape sound-symbolism may be an example of a more abstract semantic category, such as abruptness of change” and “[s]ize sound-symbolism may be an instance of a domain general magnitude symbolism that can be expressed across domains” (Kantartzis, 2011, pp. 8–9; for related discussions, see Akita, 2015 and Sidhu and Pexman, 2018). The domain-general aspect of sound symbolism gains support from the observation by Sidhu (2022) that higher order factors, such as activity, valence, potency, and novelty, may underlie various sound-symbolic associations.

3

We have no clear phonetic account of the cross-linguistically shared associations between [e] and darkness and softness. In fact, these associations are not obtained when the reference level is changed.

4

Phonotactics might also account for the limited sound-symbolic contribution of voicing in the three languages. Our stimuli had VCV forms, and the intervocalic voiceless stops pronounced by a Japanese speaker might have been perceived as voiced by speakers of these languages. Stimuli with initial obstruents (e.g., CVCV) may allow a closer examination of the sound-symbolic effect of voicing.

5

Similar examples include a soft light, a technical term in photography, meaning “a mild light that gradually fades into shadows.” Note that these synesthetic metaphors relate softness to weakness rather than to brightness itself. In fact, a hard light, another photographic expression, means “a strong light that is clearly contrasted with shadows.” Further discussion is needed on the relationship between sound symbolism and synesthetic metaphors.

6

We cannot exclude the possibility that a semantic scale that was not tested in our experiment metaphorically (or metonymically) mediated the different meanings. For example, size and darkness might be somehow connected by the third concept “magnitude.”

7

An anonymous reviewer pointed out that reflective surfaces, except those of water, mercury, etc., might link hardness and brightness metonymically as with shining armor.

8

Phonological density might also relate to pluripotentiality. For example, the larger vowel inventory of English than Japanese and Mandarin can possibly limit the sound-symbolic potential of each vowel, which may be associated with more specialized meanings. We owe this point to Susanne Fuchs.

9

Japanese ideophones exhibit similar ambiguities. For example, gorogoro /ɡoɾoɡoɾo/ imitates a cat's purr, the rumble of thunder, the rumble of the stomach, and the rumble of a heavy rolling object. It is not self-evident whether these different onomatopoeic meanings instantiate a shared abstract sound–meaning mapping or are iconically motivated on their own.

1.
Akita
,
K.
(
2013
). “
Constraints on the semantic extension of onomatopoeia
,”
Pub. J. Semiot.
5
,
21
37
.
2.
Akita
,
K.
(
2015
). “
Sound symbolism
,” in
Handbook of Pragmatics, Installment 2015
, edited by
J.-O.
Östman
and
J.
Verschueren
(
Benjamins
,
Amsterdam
).
3.
Akita
,
K.
, and
McLean
,
B.
(
2021
). “
Onshōchō chikaku no nichiei taishō: Imichizu ni yoru kentō
(“A contrastive study of Japanese and English speakers' sound-symbolic perceptions: A semantic map approach)
,” in
Jikken Ninchi Gengogaku no Shinka (Progress in Experimental Cognitive Linguistics)
, edited by
K.
Shinohara
and
R.
Uno
(
Hituzi Syobo
,
Tokyo
), pp.
165
189
.
4.
Carling
,
G.
, and
Johansson
,
N.
(
2014
). “
Motivated language change: Processes involved in the growth and conventionalization of onomatopoeia and sound symbolism
,”
Acta Linguist. Hafniensia
46
,
199
217
.
5.
Christensen
,
R. H. B.
(
2022
). “
ordinal: Regression models for ordinal data
,” R package version 2022.11.16, available at https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/ordinal/index.html (Last viewed June 25, 2023).
6.
Ćwiek
,
A.
,
Fuchs
,
S.
,
Draxler
,
C.
,
Asu
,
E. L.
,
Dediu
,
D.
,
Hiovain
,
K.
,
Kawahara
,
S.
,
Koutalidis
,
S.
,
Krifka
,
M.
,
Lippus
,
P.
,
Lupyan
,
G.
,
Oh
,
G. E.
,
Paul
,
J.
,
Petrone
,
C.
,
Ridouane
,
R.
,
Reiter
,
S.
,
Schümchen
,
N.
,
Szalontai
,
A.
,
Ünal-Logacev
,
Ö.
,
Zeller
,
J.
,
Perlman
,
M.
, and
Winter
,
B.
(
2021
). “
The bouba/kiki effect is robust across cultures and writing systems
,”
Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B
377
,
20200390
.
7.
Dingemanse
,
M.
,
Schuerman
,
W.
,
Reinisch
,
E.
,
Tufvesson
,
S.
, and
Mitterer
,
H.
(
2016
). “
What sound symbolism can and cannot do: Testing the iconicity of ideophones from five languages
,”
Language
92
,
e117
e133
.
8.
D'Onofrio
,
A.
(
2014
). “
Phonetic detail and dimensionality in sound-shape correspondences: Refining the bouba-kiki paradigm
,”
Lang. Speech
57
,
367
393
.
9.
Hamano
,
S.
(
1998
).
The Sound-Symbolic System of Japanese
(
CSLI
,
Stanford, CA
).
10.
Haspelmath
,
M.
(
2003
). “
The geometry of grammatical meaning: Semantic maps and cross-linguistic comparison
,” in
The New Psychology of Language
, edited by
M.
Tomasello
(
Erlbaum
,
Mahwah
), Vol. 2, pp.
211
242
.
11.
Johansson
,
N.
(
2017
). “
Tracking linguistic primitives: The phonosemantic realization of fundamental oppositional pairs
,” in
Dimensions of Iconicity
, edited by
A.
Zirker
,
M.
Bauer
,
O.
Fischer
, and
C.
Ljungberg
(
Benjamins
,
Amsterdam
), pp.
39
62
.
12.
Johansson
,
N.
,
Anikin
,
A.
, and
Aseyev
,
N.
(
2020a
). “
Color sound symbolism in natural languages
,”
Lang. Cogn.
12
,
56
83
.
13.
Johansson
,
N. E.
,
Anikin
,
A.
,
Carling
,
G.
, and
Holmer
,
A.
(
2020b
). “
The typology of sound symbolism: Defining macro-concepts via their semantic and phonetic features
,”
Linguist. Typol.
24
,
253
310
.
14.
Joseph
,
B. D.
(
1994
). “
Modern Greek ts: Beyond sound symbolism
,” in
Sound Symbolism
, edited by
L.
Hinton
,
J.
Nichols
, and
J. J.
Ohala
(
Cambridge University Press
,
Cambridge
), pp.
222
236
.
15.
Kantartzis
,
K.
2011
). “
Children and adults' understanding and use of Sound-Symbolism in novel words
,” Ph.D. dissertation,
University of Birmingham
,
Birmingham, AL
.
16.
Kawahara
,
S.
, and
Kumagai
,
G.
(
2021
). “
What voiced obstruents symbolically represent in Japanese: Evidence from the Pokémon universe
,”
J. Jpn. Linguist.
37
,
3
24
.
17.
Kawahara
,
S.
,
Noto
,
A.
, and
Kumagai
,
G.
(
2018
). “
Sound symbolic patterns in Pokémon names
,”
Phonetica
75
,
219
244
.
18.
Kawahara
,
S.
,
Shinohara
,
K.
, and
Grady
,
J.
(
2015
). “
Iconic inferences about personality: From sounds and shapes
,” in
Iconicity: East Meets West
, edited by
M.
Hiraga
,
W. J.
Herlofsky
,
K.
Shinohara
, and
K.
Akita
(
Benjamins
,
Amsterdam
), pp.
57
69
.
19.
Kim-Renaud
,
Y.
(
1978
). “
Semantic features in phonology: Evidence from vowel harmony in Korean
,”
Kor. Linguist.
1
,
1
18
.
20.
Köhler
,
W.
(
1929
). Gestalt Psychology (Horace Liveright, New York).
21.
Kumagai
,
G.
(
2020
). “
The pluripotentiality of bilabial consonants: The images of softness and cuteness in Japanese and English
,”
Open Linguist.
6
,
693
707
.
22.
Kumagai
,
G.
,
Uno
,
R.
, and
Shinohara
,
K.
(
2022
). “
The sound-symbolic effects of consonants on food texture
,” in
The Language of Food in Japanese: Cognitive Perspectives and Beyond
, edited by
K.
Toratani
(
Benjamins
,
Amsterdam
), pp.
79
110
.
23.
Lockwood
,
G.
, and
Dingemanse
,
M.
(
2015
). “
Iconicity in the lab: A review of behavioral, developmental, and neuroimaging research into sound-symbolism
,”
Front. Psychol.
6
,
1246
.
24.
Marks
,
L. E.
(
1975
). “
On colored-hearing synesthesia: Cross-modal translations of sensory dimensions
,”
Psychol. Bull.
82
,
303
331
.
25.
McLean
,
B.
(
2021
). “
Revising an implicational hierarchy for the meanings of ideophones, with special reference to Japonic
,”
Linguist. Typol.
25
,
507
549
.
26.
Meir
,
I.
(
2010
). “
Iconicity and metaphor: Constraints on metaphorical extension of iconic forms
,”
Language
86
,
865
896
.
27.
Mok
,
P. P. K.
,
Li
,
G.
,
Li
,
J. J.
,
Ng
,
H. T. Y.
, and
Cheung
,
H.
(
2019
). “
Cross-modal association between vowels and colours: A cross-linguistic perspective
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
145
,
2265
2276
.
28.
Ohala
,
J. J.
(
1994
). “
The frequency code underlies the sound-symbolic use of voice pitch
,” in
Sound Symbolism
, edited by
L.
Hinton
,
J.
Nichols
, and
J. J.
Ohala
(
Cambridge University Press
,
Cambridge
), pp.
325
347
.
29.
Philps
,
D.
(
2023
). “
Retracing the phonesthemic {gr-/prehension}, {sm-/oral phenomena} and {sn-/nasality} relations in English to Proto-Indo-European and beyond within a semiogenetic perspective
,”
Lingua
282
,
103449
.
30.
R Core Team.
(
2023
).
R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing
(
R Foundation for Statistical Computing
,
Vienna
).
31.
Ramachandran
,
V. S.
, and
Hubbard
,
E. M.
(
2001
). “
Synaesthesia: A window into perception, thought and language
,”
J. Conscious. Stud.
8
,
3
34
, available at https://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/imp/jcs/2001/00000008/00000012/1244.
32.
Rzymski
,
C.
,
Tresoldi
,
T.
,
Greenhill
,
S. J.
,
Wu
,
M.-S.
,
Schweikhard
,
N. E.
,
Koptjevskaia-Tamm
,
M.
,
Gast
,
V.
,
Bodt
,
T. A.
,
Hantgan
,
A.
,
Kaiping
,
G. A.
,
Chang
,
S.
,
Lai
,
Y.
,
Morozova
,
N.
,
Arjava
,
H.
,
Hübler
,
N.
,
Koile
,
E.
,
Pepper
,
S.
,
Proos
,
M.
,
Van Epps
,
B.
,
Blanco
,
I.
,
Hundt
,
C.
,
Monakhov
,
S.
,
Pianykh
,
K.
,
Ramesh
,
S.
,
Gray
,
R. D.
,
Forkel
,
R.
, and
List
,
J.-M.
(
2020
). “
The database of cross-linguistic colexifications, reproducible analysis of cross-linguistic polysemies
,”
Sci. Data
7
,
13
.
33.
Saji
,
N.
,
Akita
,
K.
,
Kantartzis
,
K.
,
Kita
,
S.
, and
Imai
,
M.
(
2019
). “
Cross-linguistically shared and language-specific sound symbolism in novel words elicited by locomotion videos in Japanese and English
,”
PLoS One
14
,
e0218707
.
34.
Shinohara
,
K.
, and
Kawahara
,
S.
(
2010
). “
A cross-linguistic study of sound symbolism: The image of size
,”
BLS
36
,
396
410
.
35.
Sidhu
,
D. M.
, and
Pexman
,
P. M.
(
2018
). “
Five mechanisms of sound symbolic association
,”
Psychon. Bull. Rev.
25
,
1619
1643
.
36.
Sidhu
,
D. M.
,
Vigliocco
,
G.
, and
Pexman
,
P. M.
(
2022
). “
Higher order factors of sound symbolism
,”
J. Mem. Lang.
125
,
104323
.
37.
Shih
,
S. S.
,
Ackerman
,
J.
,
Hermalin
,
N.
,
Inkelas
,
S.
,
Jang
,
H.
,
Johnson
,
J.
,
Kavitskaya
,
D.
,
Kawahara
,
S.
,
Oh
,
M.
,
Starr
,
R. L.
, and
Yu
,
A.
(
2019
). “
Cross-linguistic and language-specific sound symbolism: Pokémonastics,” available at
https://ling.auf.net/lingbuzz/004725 (Last viewed April 14, 2024).
38.
Sohn
,
H.
(
1999
).
The Korean Language
(
Cambridge University Press
,
Cambridge, UK
).
39.
Thompson
,
A. L.
,
Van Hoey
,
T.
, and
Do
,
Y.
(
2021
). “
Articulatory features of phonemes pattern to iconic meanings: Evidence from cross-linguistic ideophones
,”
Cogn. Linguist.
32
,
563
608
.
40.
Van Hoey
,
T.
(
2024
). “
A semantic map for ideophones
,” in
Handbook of Cognitive Semantics
, edited by
T. F.
Li
(
Brill
,
Leiden
), Chap. 16.
41.
Westbury
,
C.
,
Hollis
,
G.
,
Sidhu
,
D. M.
, and
Pexman
,
P. M.
(
2018
). “
Weighing up the evidence for sound symbolism: Distributional properties predict cue strength
,”
J. Mem. Lang.
99
,
122
150
.
42.
Winter
,
B.
(
2019
).
Sensory Linguistics: Language, Perception and Metaphor
(
Benjamins
,
Amsterdam
).
43.
Winter
,
B.
,
Oh
,
G. E.
,
Hübscher
,
I.
,
Idemaru
,
K.
,
Brown
,
L.
,
Prieto
,
P.
, and
Grawunder
,
S.
(
2021
). “
Rethinking the frequency code: A meta-analytic review of the role of acoustic body size in communicative phenomena
,”
Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B
376
,
20200400
.
44.
Winter
,
B.
,
Pérez-Sobrino
,
P.
, and
Brown
,
L.
(
2019
). “
The sound of soft alcohol: Crossmodal associations between interjections and liquor
,”
PLoS One
14
,
e0220449
.
45.
Winter
,
B.
,
Sóskuthy
,
M.
,
Perlman
,
M.
, and
Dingemanse
,
M.
(
2022
). “
Trilled /r/ is associated with roughness, linking sound and touch across spoken languages
,”
Sci. Rep.
12
,
1035
.