Dolphins are hypothesized to deduce the swimming direction of group members by attending to the spectral pattern of whistle harmonics. This is known as the direction of movement cue hypothesis and may facilitate coordination of complex group behavior when visibility is poor. The direction of movement cue hypothesis hinges on the assumption that dolphins can discriminate between whistles with different harmonic patterns that are associated with signaler orientation. This assumption was tested with a bottlenose dolphin. Whistles were recorded from a dolphin at different azimuth positions (0° to 180° in 45° increments). Noise-free, synthetic whistles were created to mimic the direction-dependant spectral profiles of the recorded whistles. A dolphin was then tested in its ability to discriminate between the synthetic whistles using fixed level and roving level conditions. The dolphin's discrimination performance in both the fixed and roving level conditions was near 100% for whistles separated by angles greater than 45°, and near chance for 45° separations. Computer simulations of the task, along with the dolphin's performance, suggest that the dolphin's discrimination was level invariant and based on the spectral pattern of the whistles.

1.
Au
,
W. W. L.
(
1992
). “
Target sonar discrimination cues
,” in
Marine Mammal Sensory Systems
, edited by
J. A.
Thomas
,
R. A.
Kastelein
, and
A. Y.
Supin
(
Plenum Press
,
New York
), pp.
357
376
.
2.
Au
,
W. W. L.
(
1993
).
The Sonar of Dolphins
(
Springer
,
New York
), pp.
1
277
.
3.
Au
,
W. W. L.
,
Andersen
,
L. N.
,
Rasmussen
,
A. R.
,
Roitblat
,
H. L.
, and
Nachtigall
,
P. E.
(
1995
). “
Neural network modeling of a dolphin's sonar discrimination capabilities
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
98
,
43
50
.
4.
Au
,
W. W. L.
,
Branstetter
,
B. K.
,
Benoit-Bird
,
K. J.
, and
Kastelein
,
R. A.
(
2009
). “
Acoustic basis for fish prey discrimination by echolocating dolphins and porpoises
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
126
,
460
467
.
5.
Au
,
W. W. L.
, and
Moore
,
P. W. B.
(
1990
). “
Critical ratio and critical bandwidth for the Atlantic bottlenose dolphin
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
88
,
1635
1638
.
6.
Benoit-Bird
,
K. J.
, and
Au
,
W. W. L.
(
2009
). “
Phonation behavior of cooperatively foraging spinner dolphins
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
125
,
539
546
.
7.
Berg
,
B. G.
(
1996
). “
On the relationship between comodulation masking release and temporal modulation transfer functions
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
100
,
1013
1023
.
8.
Blomqvist
,
C.
, and
Amundin
,
M.
(
2004
). “
High-frequency burst-pulse sounds in agonistic/aggressive interactions in bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus
,” in
Echolocation in Bats and Dolphins
, edited by
J. A.
Thomas
,
C. F.
Moss
, and
M.
Vater
(
University of Chicago Press
,
Chicago, IL
), pp.
425
431
.
9.
Branstetter
,
B. K.
, and
Finneran
,
J. J.
(
2008
). “
Comodulation masking release in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
124
,
625
633
.
10.
Branstetter
,
B. K.
,
Mercado
,
E.
, III
, and
Au
,
W. W. L.
(
2007
). “
Representing multiple discrimination cues in a computational model of the bottlenose dolphin auditory system
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
122
,
2459
2468
.
11.
Branstetter
,
B. K.
,
Moore
,
P. W.
,
Finneran
,
J. J.
,
Tormey
,
M. N.
, and
Aihara
,
H.
(
2012
). “
Directional properties of bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) clicks, burst-pulse, and whistle sounds
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
131
,
1613
1621
.
12.
Caldwell
,
M. C.
,
Caldwell
,
D. K.
, and
Tyack
,
T. L.
(
1990
). “
Review of the signature-whistle hypothesis for the Atlantic Bottlenose Dolphin
,” in
The Bottlenose Dolphin
, edited by
S.
Leatherwood
and
R. R.
Reeves
(
Academic Press, Inc.
,
San Diego, CA
), pp.
199
234
.
13.
Conner
,
R. C.
(
2000
). “
Group living in whales and dolphins
,” in
Cetacean Societies: Field Studies of Dolphins and Whales
, edited by
J.
Mann
,
R. C.
Conner
,
P. L.
Tyack
, and
H.
Whitehead
(
University of Chicago Press
,
Chicago, IL
), pp.
199
218
.
14.
Dooling
,
R. J.
, and
Okanoya
,
K.
(
1995
). “
Psychophysical methods for assessing perceptual categories
,” in
Methods in Comparative Psychoacoustics
, edited by
G. M.
Klump
,
R. J.
Dooling
,
R. R.
Fay
, and
W. C.
Stebbins
(
Birkhäuser Verlag
,
Basel, Switzerland
), pp.
307
318
.
15.
Evans
,
W. E.
(
1973
). “
Echolocation by marine delphinids and one species of freshwater dolphin
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
54
,
191
199
.
16.
Evans
,
W. E.
, and
Prescott
,
J. H.
(
1962
). “
Observations of the sound production capabilities of the Bottlenose Porpoise: A study of whistles and clicks
,”
Zoologica
47
,
121
128
.
17.
Finneran
,
J. J.
, and
Schlundt
,
C. E.
(
2011
). “
Subjective loudness level measurements and equal loudness contours in a bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
130
,
3124
3136
.
18.
Finneran
,
J. J.
,
Schlundt
,
C. E.
,
Carder
,
D. A.
, and
Ridgway
,
S. H.
(
2002
). “
Auditory filter shapes for the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) and the white whale (Delphinapterus leucas) derived with notched noise
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
112
,
322
328
.
19.
Gellerman
,
L. W.
(
1933
). “
Chance orders of alternating stimuli in visual discrimination experiments
,”
J. Genetic Psychol.
42
,
206
208
.
20.
Gillman
,
B.
(Ed). (
2000
). “
Perceptual constancies
,” in
Encyclopedia of Psychology
, edited by
A. E.
Kazdin
(
American Psychological Association
,
Washington, DC
), pp.
89
93
.
21.
Harley
,
H. E.
, and
DeLong
,
C. M.
(
2008
). “
Echoic object recognition by the bottlenose dolphin
,”
Comparative Cogn. Behav. Rev.
3
,
46
65
.
22.
Herman
,
L. M.
,
Richards
,
D. G.
, and
Wolz
,
J. P.
(
1984
). “
Comprehension of sentences by bottlenosed dolphins
,”
Cognition
16
,
129
219
.
23.
Hernandez
,
E. N.
,
Solangi
,
M.
, and
Kuczaj
,
S. A.
, II
(
2010
). “
Time and frequency parameters of bottlenose dolphin whistles as predictors of surface behavior in the Mississippi Sound
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
127
,
3232
3238
.
24.
Houser
,
D. S.
, and
Finneran
,
J. J.
(
2006
). “
Variation in the hearing sensitivity of a dolphin population obtained through the use of evoked potential audiometry
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
120
,
4090
4099
.
25.
Janik
,
V. M.
(
2000a
). “
Food-related bray calls in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
,”
Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B
267
,
923
927
.
26.
Janik
,
V. M.
(
2000b
). “
Source levels and the estimated active space of bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) whistles in the Moray Firth, Scotland
,”
J. Comp. Physiol. A
186
,
673
680
.
27.
Janik
,
V. M.
(
2000c
). “
Whistle matching in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
,”
Science
289
,
1355
1357
.
28.
Janik
,
V. M.
,
Dehnhardt
,
G.
, and
Todt
,
D.
(
1994
). “
Signature whistle variations in a bottlenosed dolphin, Tursiops truncatus
,”
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
35
,
243
248
.
29.
Janik
,
V. M.
,
Sayigh
,
L. S.
, and
Wells
,
R. S.
(
2006
). “
Signature whistle shape conveys identity information to bottlenose dolphins
,”
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
103
,
8293
8297
.
30.
Janik
,
V. M.
, and
Slater
,
P. J. B.
(
1998
). “
Context-specific use suggests that bottlenose dolphin signature whistles are cohesion calls
,”
Anim. Behav.
56
,
829
838
.
31.
Johnson
,
C. S.
(
1968a
). “
Relation between absolute threshold and duration-of-tone pulses in the bottlenosed porpoise
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
43
,
757
763
.
32.
Johnson
,
C. S.
(
1968b
). “
Sound detection thresholds in marine mammals
,”
Mar. Bioacoust.
2
,
247
260
.
33.
Kidd
,
G. J.
,
Uchanski
,
R. M.
,
Mason
,
C. R.
, and
Deliwala
,
P. S.
(
1993
). “
Discriminability of narrow-band sounds in the absence of level cues
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
93
,
1028
1037
.
34.
Lammers
,
M. O.
, and
Au
,
W. W. L.
(
2003
). “
Directionality in the whistles of Hawaiian spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris): A signal feature to cue direction of movement?
,”
Mar. Mammal Sci.
19
,
249
264
.
35.
Lammers
,
M. O.
,
Au
,
W. W. L.
, and
Herzing
,
D. L.
(
2003
). “
The broadband social acoustic signaling behavior of spinner and spotted dolphins
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
114
,
1629
1639
.
36.
Lemonds
,
D. W.
(
1999
). “
Auditory filter shapes in an Atlantic bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)
,” in
Psychology
(
University of Hawaii
,
Manoa
), p.
74
.
37.
Lilly
,
J. C.
, and
Miller
,
A. M.
(
1961
). “
Sounds emitted by the bottlenose dolphin
,”
Science
133
,
1689
1693
.
38.
May-Collado
,
L. J.
, and
Wartzok
,
D.
(
2009
). “
A characterization of Guyana dolphin (Sotalia guianensis) whistles from Costa Rica: The importance of broadband recording systems
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
125
,
1202
1213
.
39.
Miller
,
P. J. O.
(
2002
). “
Mixed-directionality of killer whale stereotyped calls: A direction of movement cue?
,”
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
52
,
262
270
.
40.
Mulsow
,
J.
, and
Finneran
,
J. J.
(
2011
). “
New approaches for studying the perception of vocal signals in otariid pinnipeds
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
129
,
2504
.
41.
Overstrom
,
N. A.
(
1983
). “
Association between burst-pulse sounds and aggressive behavior in captive Atlantic bottlenosed dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
,”
Zoo Biol.
2
,
93
103
.
42.
Pack
,
A. A.
,
Herman
,
L. M.
,
Hoffmann-Kuhnt
,
M.
, and
Branstetter
,
B. K.
(
2002
). “
The object behind the echo: Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) perceive object shape globally through echolocation
,”
Behav. Process.
58
,
1
26
.
43.
Pols
,
L. C. W.
(
1997
). “
Flexible, robust, and efficient human speech recognition
,”
Proc. Inst. Phonetic Sci.
21
,
1
10
.
44.
Quintana-Rizzo
,
E.
,
Mann
,
D. A.
, and
Wells
,
R. S.
(
2006
). “
Estimated communication range of social sounds used by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
120
,
1671
1683
.
45.
R Development Core Team (
2012
). “A language and environment for statistical computing,” (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria).
46.
Ralston
,
J. V.
, and
Herman
,
L. M.
(
1995
). “
Perception and generalization of frequency contours by a bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)
,”
J. Comparative Psychol.
109
,
268
277
.
47.
Regan
,
M. P.
(
1994
). “
Linear half-wave rectification of modulated sinusoids
,”
Appl. Math. Comput.
62
,
61
79
.
48.
Richards
,
D. G.
,
Wolz
,
J. P.
, and
Herman
,
L. M.
(
1984
). “
Vocal mimicry of computer-generated sounds and vocal labeling of objects by the bottlenosed dolphin, Tursiops truncatus
,”
J. Comparative Psychol.
98
,
10
28
.
49.
Ridgway
,
S. H.
,
Carder
,
D. A.
, and
Romano
,
T. A.
(
1991
). “
The victory squeal of dolphins and white whales on the surface and at 100 m or more in depth (A)
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
90
,
2335
.
50.
Roitblat
,
H. L.
,
Ketten
,
D.
,
Au
,
W. W. L.
, and
Nachtigall
,
P. E.
(
1996
). “
A computational model of early stages of dolphin hearing (A)
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
100
,
2643
.
51.
Slaney
,
M.
(
1993
). “
An efficient implementation of the Patterson-Holdsworth filter bank
,” Apple Comput. Tech. Rep. No. 35 (Apple Computer Library, Cupertino, CA).
52.
Smolker
,
R. A.
,
Mann
,
J.
, and
Smuts
,
B. B.
(
1993
). “
Use of signature whistles during separations and reunions by wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and infants
,”
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
33
,
393
402
.
53.
Thompson
,
R. K. R.
, and
Herman
,
L. M.
(
1975
). “
Underwater frequency discrimination in the bottlenosed dolphin (1–140 kHz) and the human (1–8 kHz)
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
57
,
943
948
.
54.
Trickey
,
J. S.
,
Branstetter
,
B. B.
, and
Finneran
,
J. J.
(
2011
). “
Auditory masking with environmental, comodulated, and Gaussian noise in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
128
,
3799
3804
.
55.
Tyack
,
P. L.
(
1997
). “
Development and social functions of signature whistles in bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus
,”
Bioacoustics
8
,
21
46
.
56.
Viemeister
,
N. F.
(
1979
). “
Temporal modulation transfer functions based upon modulation thresholds
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
66
,
1364
1380
.
57.
Yuen
,
M. M. L.
,
Nachtigall
,
P. E.
,
Breese
,
M.
, and
Vlachos
,
S. A.
(
2007
). “
The perception of complex tones by a false killer whale (Pseudorca crassidens)
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
121
,
1768
1774
.
You do not currently have access to this content.