A number of studies have examined the acoustic differences between infant-directed speech (IDS) and adult-directed speech, suggesting that the exaggerated acoustic properties of IDS might facilitate infants’ language development. However, there has been little empirical investigation of the acoustic properties that infants use for word learning. The goal of this study was thus to examine how 19-month-olds’ word recognition is affected by three acoustic properties of IDS: slow speaking rate, vowel hyper-articulation, and wide pitch range. Using the intermodal preferential looking procedure, infants were exposed to half of the test stimuli (e.g., Where’s the book?) in typical IDS style. The other half of the stimuli were digitally altered to remove one of the three properties under investigation. After the target word (e.g., book) was spoken, infants’ gaze toward target and distractor referents was measured frame by frame to examine the time course of word recognition. The results showed that slow speaking rate and vowel hyper-articulation significantly improved infants’ ability to recognize words, whereas wide pitch range did not. These findings suggest that 19-month-olds’ word recognition may be affected only by the linguistically relevant acoustic properties in IDS.

1.
Bernstein Ratner
,
N.
(
1984
). “
Phonological rule usage in mother-child speech
,”
J. Phonetics
12
,
245
254
.
2.
Bernstein Ratner
,
N.
(
1986
). “
Durational cues which mark clause boundaries in mother-child speech
,”
J. Phonetics
14
,
303
309
.
3.
Bernstein Ratner
,
N.
, and
Pye
,
C.
(
1984
). “
Higher pitch in babytalk is not universal: Acoustic evidence from Quiche Mayan
,”
J. Child Lang
11
,
515
522
.
4.
Berry
,
M. D.
, and
Erickson
,
R. L.
(
1973
). “
Speaking rate: Effects on children’s comprehension of normal speech
,”
J. Speech Hear. Res.
16
,
367
374
.
5.
Boersma
,
P.
, and
Weenink
,
D.
(
2005
). PRAAT: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 4.4.07) retrieved from http://www.praat.org/ (Last viewed 3/1/2006).
6.
Bradlow
,
A. R.
, and
Bent
,
T.
(
2002
). “
The clear speech effect for non-native listeners
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
112
,
272
284
.
7.
Bradlow
,
A. R.
,
Kraus
,
N.
, and
Hayes
,
E.
(
2003
). “
Speaking clearly for learning-impaired children: Sentence perception in noise
,”
J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res.
46
,
80
97
.
8.
Bradlow
,
A. R.
, and
Pisoni
,
D. B.
(
1999
). “
Recognition of spoken words by native and non-native listeners: Talker-, listener-, and item-related factors
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
106
,
2074
2085
.
9.
Bradlow
,
A. R.
,
Torretta
,
G. M.
, and
Pisoni
,
D. B.
(
1996
). “
Intelligibility of normal speech I: Global and fine-grained acoustic-phonetic talker characteristics
,”
Speech Commun.
20
,
255
272
.
10.
Burnham
,
D.
,
Kitamura
,
C.
, and
Vollmer-Conna
,
U.
(
2002
). “
What’s new pussycat? On talking to animals and babies
,”
Science
296
,
1435
.
11.
Colombo
,
J.
,
Frick
,
J. E.
,
Ryther
,
J. S.
,
Coldren
,
J. T.
, and
Mitchell
,
D. W.
(
1995
). “
Infants’ detection of analogs of “motherese” in noise
,”
Merrill-Palmer Q.
41
,
104
113
.
12.
Cooper
,
R. P.
,
Abraham
,
J.
,
Berman
,
S.
, and
Staska
,
M.
(
1997
). “
The development of infants’ preference for motherese
,”
Infant Behav. Dev.
20
,
477
488
.
13.
Cooper
,
R. P.
, and
Aslin
,
R. N.
(
1990
). “
Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth
,”
Child Dev.
61
,
1584
1595
.
14.
Cooper
,
R. P.
, and
Aslin
,
R. N.
(
1994
). “
Developmental differences in infant attention to the spectral properties of infant-directed speech
,”
Child Dev.
65
,
1663
1677
.
15.
Dale
,
P. S.
, and
Fenson
,
L.
(
1996
). “
Lexical development norms for young children
,”
Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput.
28
,
125
127
.
16.
Dunn
,
J.
, and
Kendrick
,
C.
(
1982
). “
The speech of two- and three-year-olds to infant siblings: ‘Baby talk’ and the context of communication
,”
J. Child Lang
9
,
579
595
.
18.
Fernald
,
A.
(
1985
). “
Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese
,”
Infant Behav. Dev.
8
,
181
195
.
19.
Fernald
,
A.
(
2000
). “
Speech to infants as hyperspeech: Knowledge-driven processes in early word recognition
,”
Phonetica
57
,
242
254
.
20.
Fernald
,
A.
, and
Kuhl
,
P. K.
(
1987
). “
Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech
,”
Infant Behav. Dev.
10
,
279
293
.
23.
Fernald
,
A.
,
Pinto
,
J. P.
,
Swingley
,
D.
,
Weinberg
,
A.
, and
McRoberts
,
G.
(
1998
). “
Rapid gains in speed of verbal processing by infants in the second year
,”
Psychol. Sci.
9
,
228
231
.
24.
Fernald
,
A.
, and
Simon
,
T.
(
1984
). “
Expanded intonation contours in mothers’ speech to newborns
,”
Dev. Psychol.
20
,
104
113
.
25.
Fernald
,
A.
,
Taeschner
,
T.
,
Dunn
,
J.
,
Papousek
,
M.
,
De Boysson-Bardies
,
B.
, and
Fukui
,
I.
(
1989
). “
A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants
,”
J. Child Lang
16
,
477
501
.
26.
Fernald
,
A.
,
Zangl
,
R.
,
Portillo
,
A. L.
, and
Marchman
,
V. A.
(
2008
). “
Looking while listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken language comprehension by infants and young children
,” in
Developmental Psycholinguistics: On-Line Methods in Children’s language processing
, edited by
I. A.
Sekerina
,
E. M.
Fernandez
, and
H.
Clahsen
(
John Benjamins
,
Amsterdam, The Netherlands
), pp.
97
135
.
28.
Gay
,
T.
(
1978
). “
Effect of speaking rate on vowel formant movements
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
63
,
223
230
.
29.
Goldfield
,
B. A.
, and
Reznick
,
J. S.
(
1990
). “
Early lexical acquisition: Rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt
,”
J. Child Lang
17
,
171
183
.
30.
Gout
,
A.
,
Christophe
,
A.
, and
Dupoux
,
E.
(
2002
). “
Testing infants’ discrimination with the orientation latency procedure
,”
Infancy
3
,
249
259
.
31.
Grieser
,
D. L.
, and
Kuhl
,
P. K.
(
1988
). “
Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features in motherese
,”
Dev. Psychol.
24
,
14
20
.
32.
Hirsh-Pasek
,
K.
, and
Golinkoff
,
R. M.
(
1996
).
The Origins of Grammar: Evidence From Early Language Comprehension
(
MIT
,
Cambridge, MA
).
33.
Hollich
,
G.
(
2003
). SUPERCODER (Version 1.5) retrieved from http:/hincapie.psych.purdue.edu/Splitscreen/SuperCoder.dmg (Last viewed 8/1/2006).
34.
Houston
,
D. M.
, and
Jusczyk
,
P. W.
(
2000
). “
The role of talker-specific information in word segmentation by infants
,”
J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.
26
,
1570
1582
.
35.
Ingram
,
D.
(
1995
). “
The cultural basis of prosodic modifications to infants and children: A response to Fernald’s universalist theory
,”
J. Child Lang
22
,
223
233
.
36.
Jacobson
,
J. L.
,
Boersma
,
D. C.
,
Fields
,
R. B.
, and
Olson
,
K. L.
(
1983
). “
Paralinguistic features of adult speech to infants and small children
,”
Child Dev.
54
,
436
442
.
37.
Johnson
,
K.
(
2004
). “
Massive reduction in conversational American English
,” in
Spontaneous Speech: Data and Analysis. Proceedings of the 1st Session of the 10th International Symposium
, edited by
K.
Yoneyama
and
K.
Maekawa
(
The National International Institute for Japanese Language
,
Tokyo, Japan
), pp.
29
54
.
38.
Karzon
,
R. G.
(
1985
). “
Discrimination of polysyllabic sequences by one-to four-month-old infants
,”
J. Exp. Child Psychol.
39
,
326
342
.
39.
Kemler Nelson
,
D. G.
,
Hirsh-Pasek
,
K.
,
Jusczyk
,
P. W.
, and
Cassidy
,
K. W.
(
1989
). “
How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning
,”
J. Child Lang
16
,
55
68
.
40.
Kitamura
,
C.
, and
Burnham
,
D.
(
1998
). “
The infant's response to maternal vocal affect
,” in
Advances in Infancy Research
, Vol.
12
, edited by
C.
Rovee-Collier
,
L.
Lipsitt
, and
H.
Hayne
(
Ablex
,
Stamford, CT
), pp.
221
236
.
41.
Kitamura
,
C.
,
Thanavishuth
,
C.
,
Burnham
,
D.
, and
Luksaneeyanawin
,
S.
(
2001
). “
Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language
,”
Infant Behav. Dev.
24
,
372
392
.
42.
Kuhl
,
P. K.
,
Andruski
,
J. E.
,
Chistovich
,
I. A.
,
Chistovich
,
L. A.
,
Kozhevnikova
,
E. V.
,
Ryskina
,
V. L.
,
Stolyarova
,
E. I.
,
Sundberg
,
U.
, and
Lacerda
,
F.
(
1997
). “
Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants
,”
Science
277
,
684
686
.
43.
Liu
,
H. -M.
,
Kuhl
,
P. K.
, and
Tsao
,
F. -M.
(
2003
). “
An association between mothers’ speech clarity and infants’ speech discrimination skills
,”
Dev. Sci.
6
,
F1
F10
.
44.
Liu
,
H. -M.
,
Tsao
,
F. -M.
, and
Kuhl
,
P. K.
(
2005
). “
The effect of reduced vowel working space on speech intelligibility in Mandarin-speaking young adults with cerebral palsy
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
117
,
3879
3889
.
45.
Malsheen
,
B. J.
(
1980
). “
Two hypotheses for phonetic clarification in the speech of mothers to children
,” in
Child Phonology. Volume 2: Perception
, edited by
G.
Yeni-Komshian
,
J.
Kaveanagh
, and
C.
Ferguson
(
Academic
,
New York
), pp.
173
184
.
46.
Morgan
,
J. L.
(
1986
).
From Simple Input to Complex Grammar
(
MIT
,
Cambridge, MA
).
47.
Nelson
,
N. W.
(
1976
). “
Comprehension of spoken language by normal children as a function of speaking rate, sentence difficulty, and listener age and sex
,”
Child Dev.
47
,
299
303
.
48.
Newman
,
R. S.
, and
Hussain
,
I.
(
2006
). “
Changes in preference for infant-directed speech in low and moderate noise by 5-to 13-month-olds
,”
Infancy
10
,
61
76
.
49.
Payton
,
K. L.
,
Uchanski
,
R. M.
, and
Braida
,
L. D.
(
1994
). “
Intelligibility of conversational and clear speech in noise and reverberation for listeners with normal and impaired hearing
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
95
,
1581
1592
.
50.
Picheny
,
M. A.
,
Durlach
,
N. I.
, and
Braida
,
L. D.
(
1985
). “
Speaking clearly for the hard of hearing I: Intelligibility differences between clear and conversational speech
,”
J. Speech Hear. Res.
28
,
96
103
.
51.
Scherer
,
K. R.
(
1986
). “
Vocal affect expression: A review and a model for future research
,”
Psychol. Bull.
99
,
143
165
.
52.
Singh
,
L.
,
Morgan
,
J.
, and
White
,
K.
(
2004
). “
Preference and processing: The role of speech affect in early spoken word recognition
,”
J. Mem. Lang.
51
,
173
189
.
53.
Singh
,
L.
,
Morgan
,
J. L.
, and
Best
,
C. T.
(
2002
). “
Infants’ listening preferences: Baby talk or happy talk?
,”
Infancy
3
,
365
394
.
54.
Singh
,
L.
,
White
,
K.
, and
Morgan
,
J. L.
(
2008
). “
Building a word-form lexicon in the face of variable input: Influences of pitch and amplitude on early spoken word recognition
,”
Lang. Learn. Dev.
4
,
157
178
.
55.
Sjölander
,
K.
, and
Beskow
,
J.
(
2000
). “
Wavesurfer—An open source speech tool
,” in
Proceedings of the ICSLP 2000
, Beijing, China, Vol.
4
, pp.
464
467
.
56.
Smiljanić
,
R.
, and
Bradlow
,
A. R.
(
2005
). “
Production and perception of clear speech in Croatian and English
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
118
,
1677
1688
.
57.
Soderstrom
,
M.
(
2007
). “
Beyond babytalk: Re-evaluating the nature and content of speech input to preverbal infants
,”
Dev. Rev.
27
,
501
532
.
58.
Stern
,
D. N.
,
Spieker
,
S.
,
Barnett
,
R. K.
, and
MacKain
,
K.
(
1983
). “
The prosody of maternal speech: Infant age and context related changes
,”
J. Child Lang
10
,
1
15
.
59.
Swingley
,
D.
, and
Fernald
,
A.
(
2002
). “
Recognition of words referring to present and absent objects by 24-month-olds
,”
J. Mem. Lang.
46
,
39
56
.
60.
Thiessen
,
E. D.
,
Hill
,
E. A.
, and
Saffran
,
J. R.
(
2005
). “
Infant-directed speech facilitates word segmentation
,”
Infancy
7
,
53
71
.
61.
Trainor
,
L. J.
, and
Desjardins
,
R. N.
(
2002
). “
Pitch characteristics of infant-directed speech affect infants’ ability to discriminate vowels
,”
Psychon. Bull. Rev.
9
,
335
340
.
62.
Uchanski
,
R. M.
,
Choi
,
S.
,
Braida
,
L. D.
,
Reed
,
C. M.
, and
Durlach
,
N. I.
(
1996
). “
Speaking clearly for the hard of hearing IV: Further studies of the role of speaking rate
,”
J. Speech Hear. Res.
39
,
494
509
.
63.
Werker
,
J. F.
, and
McLeod
,
P.
(
1989
). “
Infant preference for both male and female infant directed talk: A developmental study of attentional and affective responsiveness
,”
Can. J. Psychol.
43
,
230
246
.
64.
Werker
,
J. F.
,
Pons
,
F.
,
Dietrich
,
C.
,
Kajikawa
,
S.
,
Fais
,
L.
, and
Amano
,
S.
(
2007
). “
Infant-directed speech supports phonetic category learning in English and Japanese
,”
Cognition
103
,
147
162
.
65.
Werker
,
J. F.
, and
Tees
,
R.
(
1984
). “
Cross-language speech perception: evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life
,”
Infant Behav. Dev.
7
,
49
63
.
67.
Zangl
,
R.
,
Klarman
,
L.
,
Thal
,
D.
,
Fernald
,
A.
, and
Bates
,
E.
(
2005
). “
Dynamics of word comprehension in infancy: Developments in timing, accuracy, and resistance to acoustic degradation
,”
J. Cogn. Dev.
6
,
179
208
.
You do not currently have access to this content.