Two artificial-language learning experiments directly compared English, French, and Dutch listeners’ use of suprasegmental cues for continuous-speech segmentation. In both experiments, listeners heard unbroken sequences of consonant-vowel syllables, composed of recurring three- and four-syllable “words.” These words were demarcated by (a) no cue other than transitional probabilities induced by their recurrence, (b) a consistent left-edge cue, or (c) a consistent right-edge cue. Experiment 1 examined a vowel lengthening cue. All three listener groups benefited from this cue in right-edge position; none benefited from it in left-edge position. Experiment 2 examined a pitch-movement cue. English listeners used this cue in left-edge position, French listeners used it in right-edge position, and Dutch listeners used it in both positions. These findings are interpreted as evidence of both language-universal and language-specific effects. Final lengthening is a language-universal effect expressing a more general (non-linguistic) mechanism. Pitch movement expresses prominence which has characteristically different placements across languages: typically at right edges in French, but at left edges in English and Dutch. Finally, stress realization in English versus Dutch encourages greater attention to suprasegmental variation by Dutch than by English listeners, allowing Dutch listeners to benefit from an informative pitch-movement cue even in an uncharacteristic position.

1.
Bagou
,
O.
,
Fougeron
,
C.
, and
Frauenfelder
,
U. H.
(
2002
). “
Contribution of prosody to the segmentation and storage of ‘words' in the acquisition of a new minilanguage
,” in
Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2002
, edited by
B.
Bel
, and
I.
Marlien
(
Association pour la promotion de la phonétique et de la linguistique
,
Aix-en-Provence, France
), pp.
159
162
.
2.
Beckman
,
M. E.
, and
Edwards
,
J.
(
1990
). “
Lengthenings and shortenings and the nature of prosodic constituency
,” in
Papers in Laboratory Phonology I: Between the Grammar and Physics of Speech
, edited by
J.
Kingston
, and
M. E.
Beckman
(
Cambridge University Press
,
Cambridge
), pp.
152
178
.
3.
Betz
,
M. A.
, and
Levin
,
J. R.
(
1982
). “
Coherent analysis-of-variance hypothesis testing strategies: A general approach
,”
J. Educ. Stat.
7
,
193
206
.
4.
Bolinger
,
D. L.
(
1978
). “
Intonation across languages
,” in
Universals of Human Language
, Phonology, Vol.
2
, edited by
J. H.
Greenberg
(
Stanford University Press
,
Stanford
), pp.
471
524
.
5.
Campbell
,
N.
, and
Beckman
,
M. E.
(
1997
). “
Stress, prominence, and spectral tilt
,” in
Intonation: Theory, Models, and Applications (Proceedings of a European Speech Communication Assoc. Workshop, September 18–20, 1997)
, edited by
A.
Botinis
,
G.
Kouroupetroglou
, and
G.
Carayiannis
(
ESCA and University of Athens Department of Informatics
,
Athens
), pp.
67
70
.
6.
Cooper
,
N.
,
Cutler
,
A.
, and
Wales
,
R.
(
2002
). “
Constraints of lexical stress on lexical access in English: Evidence from native and non-native listeners
,”
Lang Speech
45
,
207
228
.
7.
Cutler
,
A.
(
2005
). “
Lexical stress
,” in
The Handbook of Speech Perception
, edited by
D. B.
Pisoni
, and
R. E.
Remez
(
Blackwell
,
Oxford
), pp.
264
289
.
8.
Cutler
,
A.
(
2009
). “
Greater sensitivity to prosodic goodness in non-native than in native listeners (L)
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
125
,
3522
3525
.
9.
Cutler
,
A.
, and
Butterfield
,
S.
(
1992
). “
Rhythmic cues to speech segmentation: Evidence from juncture misperception
,”
J. Mem. Lang.
31
,
218
236
.
10.
Cutler
,
A.
, and
Carter
,
D. M.
(
1987
). “
The predominance of strong initial syllables in the English vocabulary
,”
Comput. Speech Lang.
2
,
133
142
.
11.
Cutler
,
A.
, and
Norris
,
D.
(
1988
). “
The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access
,”
J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.
14
,
113
121
.
12.
Cutler
,
A.
, and
Otake
,
T.
(
1994
). “
Mora or phoneme? Further evidence for language-specific listening
,”
J. Mem. Lang.
33
,
824
844
.
13.
Cutler
,
A.
, and
Pasveer
,
D.
(
2006
). “
Explaining cross-linguistic differences in effects of lexical stress on spoken-word recognition
,” in
Proceedings of the Third International Conference on Speech Prosody
, edited by
R.
Hoffman
, and
H.
Mixdorff
(
TUD
,
Dresden
), pp.
250
254
.
14.
Cutler
,
A.
,
Mehler
,
J.
,
Norris
,
D.
, and
Segui
,
J.
(
1986
). “
The syllable’s differing role in the segmentation of French and English
,”
J. Mem. Lang.
25
,
385
400
.
15.
Cutler
,
A.
,
Wales
,
R.
,
Cooper
,
N.
, and
Janssen
,
J.
(
2007
). “
Dutch listeners’ use of suprasegmental cues to English stress
,” in
Proceedings of 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences
, edited by
J.
Trouvain
and
W. J.
Barry
(
Saarbrücken
,
Germany
), pp.
1913
1916
.
16.
van Donselaar
,
W.
,
Koster
,
M.
, and
Cutler
,
A.
(
2005
). “
Exploring the role of lexical stress in lexical recognition
,”
Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A
58A
,
251
273
.
17.
Dutoit
,
T.
,
Pagel
,
V.
,
Pierret
,
N.
,
Bataille
,
F.
, and
van der Vrecken
,
O.
(
1996
). “
The MBROLA project: Towards a set of high quality speech synthesizers free of use for non commercial purposes
,” in
Proceedings of the Fourth International Conference on Spoken Lang. Processing
, edited by
H. T.
Bunnell
, and
W.
Idsardi
, pp.
1393
1396
.
18.
Fear
,
B. D.
,
Cutler
,
A.
, and
Butterfield
,
S.
(
1995
). “
The strong/weak syllable distinction in English
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
97
,
1893
1904
.
19.
Hay
,
J. S. F.
, and
Diehl
,
R. L.
(
2007
). “
Perception of rhythmic grouping: Testing the iambic/trochaic law
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
69
,
113
122
.
20.
Hayes
,
B.
(
1995
).
Metrical Stress Theory: Principles and Case Studies
(
University of Chicago Press
,
Chicago
).
21.
Johnson
,
E. K.
, and
Jusczyk
,
P. W.
(
2001
). “
Word segmentation by 8-month-olds: When speech cues count more than statistics
,”
J. Mem. Lang.
44
,
548
567
.
22.
Johnson
,
E. K.
, and
Seidl
,
A. H.
(
2009
). “
At 11months, prosody still outranks statistics
,”
Dev. Sci.
12
,
131
141
.
23.
Klatt
,
D. H.
(
1975
). “
Vowel lengthening is syntactically determined in connected discourse
,”
J. Phonetics
3
,
129
140
.
24.
Lindblom
,
B.
(
1978
). “
Final lengthening in speech and music
,” in
Nordic Prosody
, edited by
E.
Gårding
,
G.
Bruce
, and
R.
Bannert
(
Department of Linguistics, Lund University
,
Lund, Sweden
), pp.
85
100
.
25.
Mattys
,
S. L.
,
White
,
L.
, and
Melhorn
,
J. F.
(
2005
). “
Integration of multiple speech segmentation cues: A hierarchical framework
,”
J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.
134
,
477
500
.
26.
McQueen
,
J. M.
(
1998
). “
Segmentation of continuous speech using phonotactics
,”
J. Mem. Lang.
39
,
21
46
.
27.
Mirman
,
D.
,
Magnuson
,
J. S.
,
Graf Estes
,
K.
, and
Dixon
,
J. A.
(
2008
). “
The link between statistical segmentation and word learning in adults
,”
Cognition
108
,
271
280
.
28.
Newman
,
R.
,
Bernstein Ratner
,
N.
,
Jusczyk
,
A. M.
,
Jusczyk
,
P. W.
, and
Dow
,
K. A.
(
2006
). “
Infants’ early ability to segment the conversational speech signal predicts later language development: A retrospective analysis
,”
Dev. Psychol.
42
,
643
655
.
29.
Norris
,
D.
, and
McQueen
,
J. M.
(
2008
). “
Shortlist B: A Bayesian model of continuous speech recognition
,”
Psychol. Rev.
115
,
357
395
.
30.
Norris
,
D.
,
McQueen
,
J. M.
,
Cutler
,
A.
, and
Butterfield
,
S.
(
1997
). “
The possible-word constraint in the segmentation of continuous speech
,”
Cognit Psychol.
34
,
191
243
.
31.
Otake
,
T.
,
Hatano
,
G.
,
Cutler
,
A.
, and
Mehler
,
J.
(
1993
). “
Mora or syllable? Speech segmentation in Japanese
,”
J. Mem. Lang.
32
,
258
278
.
32.
Otake
,
T.
,
Hatano
,
G.
, and
Yoneyama
,
K.
(
1996
). “
Speech segmentation by Japanese listeners
,” in
Phonological Structure and Language Processing: Cross-Linguistic Studies
, edited by
T.
Otake
, and
A.
Cutler
(
Mouton de Gruyter
,
Berlin
), pp.
183
201
.
33.
Palmer
,
C.
(
1997
). “
Music performance
,”
Annu. Rev. Psychol.
48
,
115
138
.
34.
Peña
,
M.
,
Bonatti
,
L. L.
,
Nespor
,
M.
, and
Mehler
,
J.
(
2002
). “
Signal-driven computations in speech processing
,”
Science
298
,
604
607
.
35.
Reber
,
R.
, and
Perruchet
,
P.
(
2003
). “
The use of control groups in artificial grammar learning
,”
Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A
56A
,
97
115
.
36.
Saffran
,
J. R.
,
Aslin
,
R. N.
, and
Newport
,
E. L.
(
1996a
). “
Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants
,”
Science
274
,
1926
1928
.
37.
Saffran
,
J. R.
,
Newport
,
E. L.
, and
Aslin
,
R. N.
(
1996b
). “
Word segmentation: The role of distributional cues
,”
J. Mem. Lang.
35
,
606
621
.
38.
Schreuder
,
R.
, and
Baayen
,
R. H.
(
1994
). “
Prefix stripping re-revisited
,”
J. Mem. Lang.
33
,
357
375
.
39.
Scott
,
D.
(
1982
). “
Duration as a cue to the perception of a phrase boundary
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
71
,
996
1007
.
40.
Sluijter
,
A. M. C.
,
van Heuven
,
V. J.
, and
Pacilly
,
J. J. A.
(
1997
). “
Spectral balance as a cue in the perception of linguistic stress
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
101
,
503
513
.
41.
Suomi
,
K.
,
McQueen
,
J. M.
, and
Cutler
,
A.
(
1997
). “
Vowel harmony and speech segmentation in Finnish
,”
J. Mem. Lang.
36
,
422
444
.
42.
Thiessen
,
E. D.
, and
Saffran
,
J. R.
(
2003
). “
When cues collide: Use of stress and statistical cues to word boundaries by 7-to9-month-old infants
,”
Dev. Psychol.
39
,
706
716
.
43.
Thiessen
,
E. D.
, and
Saffran
,
J. R.
(
2004
). “
Spectral tilt as a cue to word segmentation in infancy and adulthood
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
66
,
779
791
.
44.
Toro
,
J. M.
,
Nespor
,
M.
,
Mehler
,
J.
, and
Bonatti
,
L. L.
(
2008
). “
Finding words and rules in a speech stream: Functional differences between vowels and consonants
,”
Psychol. Sci.
19
,
137
144
.
45.
Toro-Soto
,
J. M.
,
Rodríguez-Fornells
,
A.
, and
Sebastián-Gallés
,
N.
(
2007
). “
Stress placement and word segmentation by Spanish speakers
,”
Psicologica
28
,
167
176
.
46.
Tyler
,
M. D.
(
2006
). “
French listeners can use stress to segment words in an artificial language
,” in
Proceedings of the 11th Australasian International Conference on Speech Sci. & Tech.
, edited by
P.
Warren
, and
C. I.
Watson
(
Australasian Speech Sci. and Technol. Assoc. Inc.
,
Auckland, New Zealand
), pp.
222
227
.
47.
Vaissière
,
J.
(
1983
). “
Language-independent prosodic features
,” in
Prosody: Models and Measurements
, edited by
A.
Cutler
, and
D. R.
Ladd
(
Springer-Verlag
,
Hamburg
), pp.
53
66
.
48.
van der Hulst
,
H.
(
1999
).
Word Prosodic Systems in the Languages of Europe
(
Mouton de Gruyter
,
Berlin
).
49.
van der Lugt
,
A. H.
(
2001
). “
The use of sequential probabilities in the segmentation of speech
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
63
,
811
823
.
50.
Vroomen
,
J.
,
Tuomainen
,
J.
, and
de Gelder
,
B.
(
1998
). “
The roles of word stress and vowel harmony in speech segmentation
,”
J. Mem. Lang.
38
,
133
149
.
51.
Weber
,
A.
, and
Cutler
,
A.
(
2006
). “
First-language phonotactics in second-language listening
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
119
,
597
607
.
52.
Woodrow
,
H.
(
1909
). “
A quantitative study of rhythm: The effect of variations in intensity, rate, and duration
,”
Archives of Psychol.
14
,
1
66
.
You do not currently have access to this content.