Minimum audible angle (MAA) and minimum audible movement angle (MAMA) thresholds were measured for stimuli in horizontal, vertical, and diagonal (60°) planes. A pseudovirtual technique was employed in which signals were recorded through KEMAR’s ears and played back to subjects through insert earphones. Thresholds were obtained for wideband, high-pass, and low-pass noises. Only 6 of 20 subjects obtained wideband vertical-plane MAAs less than 10°, and only these 6 subjects were retained for the complete study. For all three filter conditions thresholds were lowest in the horizontal plane, slightly (but significantly) higher in the diagonal plane, and highest for the vertical plane. These results were similar in magnitude and pattern to those reported by Perrott and Saberi [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 87, 1728–1731 (1990)] and Saberi and Perrott [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 88, 2639–2644 (1990)], except that these investigators generally found that thresholds for diagonal planes were as good as those for the horizontal plane. The present results are consistent with the hypothesis that diagonal-plane performance is based on independent contributions from a horizontal-plane system (sensitive to interaural differences) and a vertical-plane system (sensitive to pinna-based spectral changes). Measurements of the stimuli recorded through KEMAR indicated that sources presented from diagonal planes can produce larger interaural level differences (ILDs) in certain frequency regions than would be expected based on the horizontal projection of the trajectory. Such frequency-specific ILD cues may underlie the very good performance reported in previous studies for diagonal spatial resolution. Subjects in the present study could apparently not take advantage of these cues in the diagonal-plane condition, possibly because they did not externalize the images to their appropriate positions in space or possibly because of the absence of a patterned visual field.

1.
Algazi
,
V. R.
,
Avendano
,
C.
, and
Duda
,
R. O.
(
2001
). “
Elevation localization and head-related transfer function analysis at low frequencies
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
109
,
1110
1122
.
2.
ANSI (1989). ANSI S3.6-1989, “Specifications for Audiometers” (American National Standards Institute, New York).
3.
Blauert, J. (1997). Spatial Hearing: The Psychophysics of Human Sound Localization, Revised Edition (MIT, Cambridge, MA).
4.
Chandler
,
D. W.
, and
Grantham
,
D. W.
(
1992
). “
Minimum audible movement angle in the horizontal plane as a function of stimulus frequency and bandwidth, source azimuth, and velocity
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
91
,
1624
1636
.
5.
Grantham, D. W. (1997). “Auditory motion perception: Snapshots revisited,” in Binaural and Spatial Hearing in Real and Virtual Environments, edited by R. H. Gilkey and T. R. Anderson (Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ), pp. 295–313.
6.
Grantham
,
D. W.
(
1986
). “
Detection and discrimination of simulated motion of auditory targets in the horizontal plane
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
79
,
1939
1949
.
7.
Grantham, D. W. (1995). “Spatial hearing and related phenomena,” in Handbook of Perception and Cognition: Hearing, edited by B. C. J. Moore (Academic, San Diego), pp. 297–345.
8.
Hafter, E. R., Buell, T. N., Basiji, D. A., and Shriberg, E. E. (1988). “Discrimination of direction for complex sounds presented in the free-field,” in Basic Issues in Hearing: Proceedings of the 8th International Symposium on Hearing, edited by H. Duifhuis, J. W. Horst, and H. P. Wit (Academic, London), pp. 394–401.
9.
Harris
,
J. D.
, and
Sergeant
,
R. L.
(
1971
). “
Monaural/binaural minimum audible angles for a moving sound source
,”
J. Speech Hear. Res.
14
,
618
629
.
10.
Hebrank
,
J.
, and
Wright
,
D.
(
1974
). “
Spectral cues used in the localization of sound sources on the median plane
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
56
,
1829
1834
.
11.
Koehnke
,
J.
, and
Besing
,
J. M.
(
1996
). “
A procedure for testing speech intelligibility in a virtual listening environment
,”
Ear Hear.
17
,
211
217
.
12.
Lakatos
,
S.
(
1995
). “
The influence of visual cues on the localisation of circular auditory motion
,”
Perception
24
,
457
465
.
13.
Levitt
,
H.
(
1971
). “
Transformed up–down methods in psychoacoustics
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
49
,
467
477
.
14.
Litovsky
,
R. Y.
, and
Macmillan
,
N. A.
(
1994
). “
Sound localization precision under conditions of the precedence effect: Effects of azimuth and standard stimuli
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
96
,
752
758
.
15.
Middlebrooks
,
J. C.
(
1999
). “
Virtual localization improved by scaling nonindividualized external-ear transfer functions in frequency
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
106
,
1493
1510
.
16.
Mills
,
A. W.
(
1958
). “
On the minimum audible angle
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
30
,
237
246
.
17.
Perrott
,
D. R.
, and
Marlborough
,
K.
(
1989
). “
Minimum audible movement angle: Marking the end points of the path traveled by a moving sound source
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
85
,
1773
1775
.
18.
Perrott
,
D. R.
, and
Musicant
,
A. D.
(
1977
). “
Minimum auditory movement angle: Binaural localization of moving sound sources
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
62
,
1463
1466
.
19.
Perrott
,
D. R.
, and
Pacheco
,
S.
(
1989
). “
Minimum audible angle thresholds for broadband noise as a function of the delay between the onset of the lead and lag signals
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
85
,
2669
2672
.
20.
Perrott
,
D. R.
, and
Saberi
,
K.
(
1990
). “
Minimum audible angle thresholds for sources varying in both elevation and azimuth
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
87
,
1728
1731
.
21.
Perrott
,
D. R.
, and
Tucker
,
J.
(
1988
). “
Minimum audible movement angle as a function of signal frequency and the velocity of the source
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
83
,
1522
1527
.
22.
Platt
,
B. B.
, and
Warren
,
D. H.
(
1972
). “
Auditory localization: The importance of eye movements and a textured visual environment
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
12
,
245
248
.
23.
Roffler
,
S. K.
, and
Butler
,
R. A.
(
1968
). “
Factors that influence the localization of sound in the vertical plane
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
43
,
1255
1259
.
24.
Saberi
,
K.
,
Dostal
,
L.
,
Sadralodabai
,
T.
, and
Perrott
,
D. R.
(
1991
). “
Minimum audible angles for horizontal, vertical, and oblique orientations: Lateral and dorsal planes
,”
Acustica
75
,
57
61
.
25.
Saberi
,
K.
(
1995
). “
Some considerations on the use of adaptive methods for estimating interaural-delay thresholds
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
98
,
1803
1806
.
26.
Saberi
,
K.
, and
Green
,
D. M.
(
1997
). “
Evaluation of maximum-likelihood estimators in nonintensive auditory psychophysics
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
59
,
867
876
.
27.
Saberi
,
K.
, and
Perrott
,
D. R.
(
1990
). “
Minimum audible movement angles as a function of sound source trajectory
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
88
,
2639
2644
.
28.
Shelton
,
B. R.
, and
Searle
,
C. L.
(
1980
). “
The influence of vision on the absolute identification of sound-source position
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
28
,
589
596
.
29.
Wenzel
,
E. M.
,
Arruda
,
M.
,
Kistler
,
D. J.
, and
Wightman
,
F. L.
(
1993
). “
Localization using nonindividualized head-related transfer functions
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
94
,
111
123
.
This content is only available via PDF.
You do not currently have access to this content.