Peripheral-channeling theorists argue that differences in excitation pattern between successive sounds are necessary for stream segregation to occur. The component phases of complex tones comprising unresolved harmonics (F0=100 Hz) were manipulated to change pitch and timbre without changing the power spectrum. In experiment 1, listeners compared two alternating sequences of tones, A and B. One sequence was isochronous (tone duration=60 ms, intertone interval=40 ms). The other began isochronously, but the progressive delay of tone B made the rhythm irregular. Subjects had to identify the sequence with irregular rhythm. Stream segregation makes this task more difficult. A and B could differ in passband (1250–2500 Hz, 1768–3536 Hz, 2500–5000 Hz), component phase (cosine, alternating, random), or both. Stimuli were presented at 70 dB SPL in pink noise. Dissimilarity in either passband or phase increased discrimination thresholds. Moreover, phase differences raised threshold even when there was no passband difference. In experiment 2, listeners judged moment-by-moment the grouping of long ABA-ABA-… sequences. The measure was the proportion of time a sequence was heard as segregated. The factors that increased segregation were very similar to those that increased threshold in experiment 1. Overall, the findings indicate that substantial stream segregation can occur without differences in power spectrum. It is concluded that differences in peripheral channeling are not a requirement for stream segregation.

1.
Anstis
,
S.
, and
Saida
,
S.
(
1985
). “
Adaptation to auditory streaming of frequency-modulated tones
,”
J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.
11
,
257
271
.
2.
Attneave
,
F.
(
1971
). “
Multistability in perception
,”
Sci. Am.
225
,
62
71
.
3.
Beauvois
,
M. W.
, and
Meddis
,
R.
(
1996
). “
Computer simulation of auditory stream segregation in alternating-tone sequences
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
99
,
2270
2280
.
4.
Bilsen
,
F. A.
(
1973
). “
On the influence of the number and phase of harmonics on the perceptibility of the pitch of complex signals
,”
Acustica
28
,
60
65
.
5.
Bregman
,
A. S.
(
1978
). “
Auditory streaming is cumulative
,”
J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.
4
,
380
387
.
6.
Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory Scene Analysis: The Perceptual Organization of Sound (MIT, Cambridge, MA).
7.
Bregman
,
A. S.
, and
Campbell
,
J.
(
1971
). “
Primary auditory stream segregation and perception of order in rapid sequences of tones
,”
J. Exp. Psychol.
89
,
244
249
.
8.
Bregman
,
A. S.
,
Liao
,
C.
, and
Levitan
,
R.
(
1990
). “
Auditory grouping based on fundamental frequency and formant peak frequency
,”
Can. J. Psychol.
44
,
400
413
.
9.
Broadbent
,
D. E.
, and
Ladefoged
,
P.
(
1959
). “
Auditory perception of temporal order
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
31
,
1539
.
10.
Burns
,
E. M.
, and
Viemeister
,
N. F.
(
1976
). “
Nonspectral pitch
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
60
,
863
869
.
11.
Carlyon
,
R. P.
, and
Datta
,
A. J.
(
1997
). “
Excitation produced by Schroeder-phase complexes: Evidence for fast-acting compression in the auditory system
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
101
,
3636
3647
.
12.
Carlyon
,
R. P.
,
Cusack
,
R.
,
Foxton
,
J. M.
, and
Robertson
,
I. H.
(
2001
). “
Effects of attention and unilateral neglect on auditory stream segregation
,”
J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.
27
,
115
127
.
13.
Cusack
,
R.
, and
Roberts
,
B.
(
1999
). “
Effects of similarity in bandwidth on the auditory sequential streaming of two-tone complexes
,”
Perception
28
,
1281
1289
.
14.
Cusack
,
R.
, and
Roberts
,
B.
(
2000
). “
Effects of differences in timbre on sequential grouping
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
62
,
1112
1120
.
15.
Dowling
,
W. J.
(
1973
). “
The perception of interleaved melodies
,”
Cogn. Psychol.
5
,
322
337
.
16.
Fisher
,
G. H.
(
1967
). “
Measuring ambiguity
,”
Am. J. Psychol.
80
,
541
557
.
17.
Glasberg
,
B. R.
, and
Moore
,
B. C. J.
(
1990
). “
Derivation of auditory filter shapes from notched-noise data
,”
Hear. Res.
47
,
103
138
.
18.
Gockel
,
H.
,
Moore
,
B. C. J.
, and
Patterson
,
R. D.
(
2002
). “
Influence of component phase on the loudness of complex tones
,”
Acta Acust. Acust.
88
,
369
377
.
19.
Grey
,
J. M.
(
1977
). “
Multidimensional perceptual scaling of musical timbres
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
61
,
1270
1277
.
20.
Grimault
,
N.
,
Bacon
,
S. P.
, and
Micheyl
,
C.
(
2002
). “
Auditory stream segregation on the basis of amplitude-modulation rate
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
111
,
1340
1348
.
21.
Grimault
,
N.
,
Micheyl
,
C.
,
Carlyon
,
R. P.
,
Arthaud
,
P.
, and
Collet
,
L.
(
2000
). “
Influence of peripheral resolvability on the perceptual segregation of harmonic complex tones differing in fundamental frequency
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
108
,
263
271
.
22.
Grose
,
J. H.
,
Hall
,
J. W.
,
Buss
,
E.
, and
Hatch
,
D.
(
2001
). “
Gap detection for similar and dissimilar gap markers
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
109
,
1587
1595
.
23.
Hartmann
,
W. M.
, and
Johnson
,
D.
(
1991
). “
Stream segregation and peripheral channeling
,”
Music Percept.
9
,
155
183
.
24.
Houtsma
,
A. J. M.
, and
Smurzynski
,
J.
(
1990
). “
Pitch identification and discrimination for complex tones with many harmonics
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
87
,
304
310
.
25.
Iverson
,
P.
(
1995
). “
Auditory stream segregation by musical timbre: Effects of static and dynamic acoustic attributes
,”
J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.
21
,
751
763
.
26.
Kohlrausch, A., and Houtsma, A. J. M. (1992). “Pitch related to spectral edges of broadband signals,” in Processing of Complex Sounds by the Auditory System, edited by R. P. Carlyon, C. J. Darwin, and I. J. Russell (Oxford U.P., Oxford, UK), pp. 81–88.
27.
Levitt
,
H.
(
1971
). “
Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
49
,
467
477
.
28.
McCabe
,
S. L.
, and
Denham
,
M. J.
(
1997
). “
A model of auditory streaming
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
101
,
1611
1621
.
29.
Miller
,
G. A.
, and
Heise
,
G. A.
(
1950
). “
The trill threshold
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
22
,
637
638
.
30.
Moore, B. C. J. (2002). “Frequency resolution,” in Genetics and the Function of the Auditory System, edited by L. Tranebjærg, V. Christensen-Dalsgaard, T. Andersen, and T. Poulsen (Holmens Trykkeri, Copenhagen, Denmark), pp. 227–271.
31.
Moore
,
B. C. J.
, and
Gockel
,
H.
(
2002
). “
Factors influencing sequential stream segregation
,”
Acta Acust. Acust.
88
,
320
333
.
32.
Moore
,
B. C. J.
, and
Oxenham
,
A. J.
(
1998
). “
Psychoacoustic consequences of compression in the peripheral auditory system
,”
Psychol. Rev.
105
,
108
124
.
33.
Moore
,
B. C. J.
, and
Rosen
,
S. M.
(
1979
). “
Tune recognition with reduced pitch and interval information
,”
Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
31
,
229
240
.
34.
Moore
,
B. C. J.
,
Glasberg
,
B. R.
, and
Baer
,
T.
(
1997
). “
A model for the prediction of thresholds, loudness, and partial loudness
,”
J. Audio Eng. Soc.
45
,
224
240
.
35.
Ortmann
,
O.
(
1926
). “
On the melodic relativity of tones
,”
Psychol. Monogr.
35
,
1
47
(Whole No. 162).
36.
Pressnitzer, D., and Patterson, R. D. (2001). “Distortion products and the pitch of harmonic complex tones,” in Physiological and Psychophysical Bases of Auditory Function, edited by D. J. Breebaart, A. J. M. Houtsma, A. Kohlrausch, V. F. Prijs, and R. Schoonhoven (Shaker, Maastrict, The Netherlands), pp. 84–91.
37.
Rice, S. O. (1954). “Mathematical analysis of random noise,” in Selected Papers on Noise and Stochastic Processes, edited by N. Wax (Dover, New York), pp. 133–294.
38.
Ritsma
,
R. J.
(
1962
). “
Existence region of the tonal residue. I
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
34
,
1224
1229
.
39.
Ritsma
,
R. J.
(
1963
). “
Existence region of the tonal residue. II
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
35
,
1241
1245
.
40.
Schroeder
,
M. R.
(
1970
). “
Synthesis of low-peak-factor signals and binary sequences with low autocorrelation
,”
IEEE Trans. Inf. Theory
IT-16
,
85
89
.
41.
Shackleton
,
T. M.
, and
Carlyon
,
R. P.
(
1994
). “
The role of resolved and unresolved harmonics in pitch perception and frequency modulation discrimination
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
95
,
3529
3540
.
42.
Shera
,
C. A.
,
Guinan
,
J. J.
, and
Oxenham
,
A. J.
(
2002
). “
Revised estimates of human cochlear tuning from otoacoustic and behavioral measurements
,”
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
99
,
3318
3323
.
43.
Singh
,
P. G.
(
1987
). “
Perceptual organization of complex-tone sequences: A tradeoff between pitch and timbre?
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
82
,
886
899
.
44.
Singh
,
P. G.
, and
Bregman
,
A. S.
(
1997
). “
The influence of different timbre attributes on the perceptual segregation of complex-tone sequences
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
102
,
1943
1952
.
45.
Smoorenburg
,
G. F.
(
1972
). “
Audibility region of combination tones
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
52
,
603
614
.
46.
Snedecor, G. W., and Cochran, W. G. (1967). Statistical Methods, 6th ed. (Iowa U.P., Ames, Iowa).
47.
van Noorden, L. P. A. S. (1975). “Temporal coherence in the perception of tone sequences,” doctoral thesis, Eindhoven University of Technology, The Netherlands.
48.
Vliegen
,
J.
, and
Oxenham
,
A. J.
(
1999
). “
Sequential stream segregation in the absence of spectral cues
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
105
,
339
346
.
49.
Vliegen
,
J.
,
Moore
,
B. C. J.
, and
Oxenham
,
A. J.
(
1999
). “
The role of spectral and periodicity cues in auditory stream segregation, measured using a temporal discrimination task
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
106
,
938
945
.
50.
von Bismarck
,
G.
(
1974
). “
Timbre of steady sounds: A factorial investigation of its verbal attributes
,”
Acustica
30
,
146
159
.
51.
Warren
,
R. M.
,
Obusek
,
C. J.
,
Farmer
,
R. M.
, and
Warren
,
R. P.
(
1969
). “
Auditory sequence: Confusion of patterns other than speech and music
,”
Science
164
,
586
587
.
52.
Wessel
,
D. L.
(
1979
). “
Timbre space as a musical control structure
,”
Comput. Music J.
3
,
45
52
.
This content is only available via PDF.
You do not currently have access to this content.