The present article aims at exploring the invariant parameters involved in the perceptual normalization of French vowels. A set of 490 stimuli, including the ten French vowels /i y u e ø o ɛ œ ɔ a/ produced by an articulatory model, simulating seven growth stages and seven fundamental frequency values, has been submitted as a perceptual identification test to 43 subjects. The results confirm the important effect of the tonality distance between F1 and f0 in perceived height. It does not seem, however, that height perception involves a binary organization determined by the 3–3.5-Bark critical distance. Regarding place of articulation, the tonotopic distance between F1 and F2 appears to be the best predictor of the perceived front–back dimension. Nevertheless, the role of the difference between F2 and F3 remains important. Roundedness is also examined and correlated to the effective second formant, involving spectral integration of higher formants within the 3.5-Bark critical distance. The results shed light on the issue of perceptual invariance, and can be interpreted as perceptual constraints imposed on speech production.

1.
Ainsworth
,
W. A.
(
1971
). “
Perception of synthesized isolated vowels and h_d words as a function of fundamental frequency
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
49
,
1323
1324
.
2.
Ainsworth, W. A. (1975). “Intrinsic and Extrinsic Factors in Vowel Judgements,” in Auditory Analysis and Perception of Speech, edited by G. Fant and M. A. A. Tatham (Academic, London), pp. 103–113.
3.
Badin
,
P.
, and
Fant
,
G.
(
1984
). “
Notes on vocal tract computations
,”
STL QPSR
2–3
,
53
108
.
4.
Beck, J. M. (1996). “Organic variation of the vocal apparatus,” in Handbook of Phonetic Sciences, edited by W. J. Hardcastle and J. Laver (Blackwell, London), pp. 256–297.
5.
Bladon
,
R. A. W.
, and
Fant
,
G.
(
1978
). “
A two-formant model and the cardinal vowels
,”
STL-QPSR
1
,
1
8
.
6.
Boë, L.-J., and Maeda, S. (1997). “Modélisation de la croissance du conduit vocal. Espace vocalique des nouveaux-nés et des adultes. Conséquences pour l’ontogenèse et la phylogenèse,” Journées d’Études Linguistiques: “La Voyelle dans Tous ces États,” Nantes, pp. 98–105.
7.
Boë, L.-J., Perrier, P., Guérin, B., and Schwartz, J.-L. (1989). “Maximal Vowel Space,” in European Conference on Speech Communication and Technology (Eurospeech), Paris, France, pp. 281–284.
8.
Carlson
,
R.
,
Granström
,
B.
, and
Fant
,
G.
(
1970
). “
Some studies concerning perception of isolated vowels
,”
STL-QPSR
2–3
,
19
35
.
9.
Carlson
,
R.
,
Granström
,
B.
, and
Klatt
,
D.
(
1979
). “
Vowel perception: The relative salience of selected acoustic manipulations
,”
STL-QPSR
34
,
19
35
.
10.
Chistovich, L. A., Sheikin, R. L., and Lublinskaya, V. V. (1979). “Centres of Gravity’ and Spectral Peaks as the Determinants of Vowel Quality,” in Frontiers of Speech Communication Research, edited by B. Lindblom and S. Öhman (Academic, London), pp. 143–157.
11.
Delattre
,
P.
,
Liberman
,
A. M.
,
Cooper
,
F. S.
, and
Gertsman
,
J.
(
1952
). “
An experimental study of the acoustic determinants of vowel color; observations on one- and two-formant vowels synthesized from spectrographic patterns
,”
Word
8
,
195
210
12.
Diehl
,
R. L.
(
2000
). “
Searching for an Auditory Description of Vowel Categories
,”
Phonetica
57
,
267
274
.
13.
Fahey
,
R. P.
,
Diehl
,
R. L.
, and
Traunmüller
,
H.
(
1996
). “
Perception of back vowels: effects of varying F1-F0 Bark distance
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
99
,
2350
2357
.
14.
Fant
,
G.
(
1983
). “
Feature analysis of Swedish vowels—A revisit
,”
STL-QPSR
2–3
,
1
19
.
15.
Fant, G., Carlson R., and Granström, B. (1974). “The [e]-[ø] ambiguity,” in Proceedings of Speech Communication Seminar, Stockholm, pp. 117–121.
16.
Fujisaki
,
H.
, and
Kawashima
,
T.
(
1968
). “
The Roles of Pitch and Higher Formants in the Perception of Vowels
,”
IEEE Trans. Audio Electroacoust.
AU-16
(
1
),
73
77
.
17.
Goldstein, U. G. (1980). “An articulatory model for the vocal tract of the growing children,” Thesis of Doctor of Science, MIT, Cambridge, MA.
18.
Hillenbrand
,
J.
,
Getty
,
L. A.
,
Clark
,
M. J.
, and
Wheeler
,
K.
(
1995
). “
Acoustic characteristics of American English vowels
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
97
,
3099
3111
.
19.
Hirahara, T., and Kato, H. (1992). “The Effect of F0 on Vowel Identification,” in Speech Perception, Production and Linguistic Structure, edited by Y. Tohkura, E. Vatikiotis-Bateson, and Y. Sagisaka (Ohmsha/IOS, Tokyo), pp. 89–112.
20.
Hoemeke
,
K. A.
, and
Diehl
,
R. L.
(
1994
). “
Perception of vowel height: The role of F1-F0 distance
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
96
,
661
674
.
21.
Jordan
,
M. I.
, and
Rumelhart
,
D. E.
(
1992
). “
Forward Models: Supervised Learning with a Distal Teacher
,”
Cogn. Sci.
16
,
316
354
.
22.
Kent, R. D. (1992). “The Biology of Phonological Development,” in Phonological Development: Models, Research, Implications, edited by C. A. Ferguson, L. Menn, and C. Stoel-Gammon (York, Timonium, MD), pp. 65–90.
23.
Kuhl
,
P. K.
, and
Meltzoff
,
A. N.
(
1996
). “
Infant vocalizations in response to speech: Vocal imitations and developmental change
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
100
,
2425
2438
.
24.
Lee
,
S.
,
Potamianos
,
A.
, and
Narayanan
,
S.
(
1999
). “
Acoustics of children’s speech: Developmental changes of temporal and spectral parameters
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
105
,
1455
1468
.
25.
Liberman
,
A. M.
, and
Mattingly
,
I. G.
(
1985
). “
The motor theory of speech perception revisited
,”
Cognition
21
,
1
36
.
26.
Lindblom
,
B.
(
1996
). “
Role of articulation in speech perception: Clues from production
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
99
,
1683
1692
.
27.
Lotto
,
A. J.
,
Holt
,
L. L.
, and
Kluender
,
K. R.
(
1997
). “
Effect of Voice Quality on Perceived Height of English Vowels
,”
Phonetica
54
,
76
93
.
28.
Mantakas, M. (1989). “Application du second formant effectif F’2 à l’étude de l’opposition d’arrondissement des voyelles antérieures du français,” Thèse de Docteur de l’INPG, Systèmes Electroniques, Grenoble.
29.
Ménard, L., and Boë, L.-J. (2000). “Exploring Vowel Production Strategies from Infant to Adult by Means of Articulatory Inversion of Formant Data,” in International Congress of Spoken Language Processing, Beijing, China, pp. 465–468.
30.
Ménard, L., and Boë, L.-J. (2001). “Perceptual categorization of maximal vowel space from birth to adulthood,” in European Conference on Speech Communication and Technology (Eurospeech), Aalborg, Denmark, pp. 167–170.
31.
Miller
,
D. C.
(
1953
). “
Auditory tests with synthetic vowels
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
25
,
114
121
.
32.
Miller
,
J. D.
(
1989
). “
Auditory-perceptual interpretation of the vowel
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
85
,
2114
2134
.
33.
Molis, M. (1999). “Perception of vowel quality in the F2/F3 plane,” in Proceedings ICPhS 99, San Francisco, pp. 171–194.
34.
Nearey
,
T. M.
(
1989
). “
Static, dynamic, and relational properties in vowel perception
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
85
,
2088
2113
.
35.
Peterson
,
G. E.
, and
Barney
,
H. L.
(
1952
). “
Control method used in the study of vowels
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
24
,
175
184
.
36.
Potter
,
R. K.
, and
Steinberg
,
J. C.
(
1950
). “
Toward the specification of speech
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
22
,
807
820
.
37.
Savariaux
,
C.
,
Perrier
,
P.
,
Orliaguet
,
J.-P.
, and
Schwartz
,
J.-L.
(
1999
). “
Compensation strategies for the perturbation of French [u] using a lip tube. II. Perceptual analysis
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
106
,
381
393
.
38.
Schroeder, M. R., Atal, B. S., and Hall, J. L. (1979). “Objective measure of certain speech signal degradations based on masking properties of human auditory perception,” in Frontiers of Speech Communication Research, edited by B. Lindblom and S. Öhman (Academic, London), pp. 217–229.
39.
Schwartz
,
J.-L.
,
Beautemps
,
D.
,
Abry
,
C.
, and
Escudier
,
P.
(
1993
). “
Inter-individual and cross-linguistic strategies for the production of the [i] vs [y] contrast
,”
J. Phonetics
21
,
411
425
.
40.
Schwartz
,
J.-L.
,
Boë
,
L.-J.
,
Vallée
,
N.
, and
Abry
,
C.
(
1997
). “
The Dispersion-Focalization Theory of vowel systems
,”
J. Phonetics
25
,
255
286
.
41.
Slawson
,
A. W.
(
1968
). “
Vowel quality and musical timbre as functions of spectrum envelope and fundamental frequency
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
43
,
87
101
.
42.
Stevens
,
K. N.
(
1996
). “
Critique: Articulatory-acoustic relations and their role in speech perception
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
99
,
1693
1694
.
43.
Strange
,
W.
(
1989
). “
Dynamic aspects of coarticulated vowels spoken in sentence context
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
85
,
2135
2153
.
44.
Syrdal
,
A. K.
, and
Gopal
,
H. S.
(
1986
). “
A perceptual model of vowel recognition based on the auditory representation of American English vowels
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
79
,
1086
1100
.
45.
Traunmüller
,
H.
(
1981
). “
Perceptual dimension of openness in vowels
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
69
,
1465
1475
.
46.
Traunmüller
,
H.
(
1984
). “
Articulatory and perceptual factors controlling the age- and sex-conditioned variability in formant frequencies of vowels
,”
Speech Commun.
3
,
49
61
.
47.
Traunmüller, H. (1991). “The context sensitivity of the perceptual interaction between F0 and F1,” in Proceedings of the XIIth ICPhS, Aix-en-Provence, France, Vol. 5, pp. 62–65.
48.
Traunmüller
,
H.
, and
Lacerda
,
F.
(
1987
). “
Perceptual relativity in identification of two-formant vowels
,”
Speech Commun.
6
,
143
157
.
49.
Vallée, N. (1994). “Systèmes vocaliques: de la typologie aux prédictions,” Thèse de Doctorat en Sciences du Language, Université Stendhal, Grenoble.
This content is only available via PDF.
You do not currently have access to this content.