Studies with adults have demonstrated that acoustic cues cohere in speech perception such that two stimuli cannot be discriminated if separate cues bias responses equally, but oppositely, in each. This study examined whether this kind of coherence exists for children’s perception of speech signals, a test that first required that a contrast be found for which adults and children show similar cue weightings. Accordingly, experiment 1 demonstrated that adults, 7-, and 5-year-olds weight F2-onset frequency and gap duration similarly in “spa” versus “sa” decisions. In experiment 2, listeners of these same ages made “same” or “not-the-same” judgments for pairs of stimuli in an AX paradigm when only one cue differed, when the two cues were set within a stimulus to bias the phonetic percept towards the same category (relative to the other stimulus in the pair), and when the two cues were set within a stimulus to bias the phonetic percept towards different categories. Unexpectedly, adults’ results contradicted earlier studies: They were able to discriminate stimuli when the two cues conflicted in how they biased phonetic percepts. Results for 7-year-olds replicated those of adults, but were not as strong. Only the results of 5-year-olds revealed the kind of perceptual coherence reported by earlier studies for adults. Thus, it is concluded that perceptual coherence for speech signals is present from an early age, and in fact listeners learn to overcome it under certain conditions.

1.
ANSI (1989). “Specifications for Audiometers” (American National Standards Institute, New York).
2.
Best
,
C. T.
,
Morrongiello
,
B. A.
, and
Robson
,
R. C.
(
1981
). “
Perceptual equivalence of acoustic cues in speech and nonspeech perception
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
29
,
191
211
.
3.
Best
,
C. T.
,
Studdert-Kennedy
,
M.
,
Manuel
,
S.
, and
Rubin-Spitz
,
J.
(
1989
). “
Discovering phonetic coherence in acoustic patterns
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
45
,
237
250
.
4.
Blumstein
,
S. E.
, and
Stevens
,
K. N.
(
1980
). “
Perceptual invariance and onset spectra for stop consonants in different vowel environments
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
67
,
648
662
.
5.
Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory Scene Analysis: The Perceptual Organization of Sound (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA).
6.
Broadbent
,
D. E.
, and
Ladefoged
,
P.
(
1957
). “
On the fusion of sounds reaching different sense organs
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
29
,
708
710
.
7.
Eilers
,
R. E.
,
Oller
,
D. K.
,
Urbano
,
R.
, and
Moroff
,
D.
(
1989
). “
Conflicting and cooperating cues: Perception of cues to final consonant voicing by infants and adults
,”
J. Speech Hear. Res.
32
,
307
316
.
8.
Fitch
,
H.
,
Halwes
,
T.
,
Erickson
,
D.
, and
Liberman
,
A.
(
1980
). “
Perceptual equivalence of two acoustic cues for stop-consonant manner
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
27
,
343
350
.
9.
Goldman, R., and Fristoe, M. (1986). Goldman Fristoe Test of Articulation (American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, MN).
10.
Greenlee
,
M.
(
1980
). “
Learning the phonetic cues to the voiced-voiceless distinction: A comparison of child and adult speech perception
,”
J. Child Lang.
7
,
459
468
.
11.
Holt
,
L. L.
,
Lotto
,
A. J.
, and
Kluender
,
K. R.
(
2001
). “
Influence of fundamental frequency on stop-consonant voicing perception: A case of learned covariation or auditory enhancement?
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
109
,
764
774
.
12.
Jastak, S., and Wilkinson, G. S. (1984). The Wide Range Achievement Test—Revised (Jastak Associates, Wilmington, DE).
13.
Kewley-Port
,
D.
(
1983
). “
Time-varying features as correlates of place of articulation in stop consonants
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
73
,
322
335
.
14.
Kluender
,
K. R.
,
Lotto
,
A. J.
,
Holt
,
L. L.
, and
Bloedel
,
S. L.
(
1998
). “
Role of experience in language-specific functional mappings for vowel sounds as inferred from human, nonhuman and computational models
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
104
,
3568
3582
.
15.
Krause
,
S. E.
(
1982
). “
Vowel duration as a perceptual cue to postvocalic consonant voicing in young children and adults
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
71
,
990
995
.
16.
Liberman
,
A. M.
(
1957
). “
Some results of research on speech perception
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
29
,
117
123
.
17.
Liberman
,
A. M.
,
Cooper
,
F. S.
,
Shankweiler
,
D. P.
, and
Studdert-Kennedy
,
M.
(
1967
). “
Perception of the speech code
,”
Psychol. Rev.
74
,
431
461
.
18.
Liberman
,
A. M.
,
Isenberg
,
D.
, and
Rakerd
,
B.
(
1981
). “
Duplex perception of cues for stop consonants: Evidence for a phonetic mode
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
30
,
133
143
.
19.
Mann
,
V. A.
, and
Liberman
,
A. M.
(
1983
). “
Some differences between phonetic and auditory modes of perception
,”
Cognition
14
,
211
235
.
20.
Mann
,
V. A.
, and
Repp
,
B. H.
(
1980
). “
Influence of vocalic context on perception of the [∫]–[s] distinction
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
28
,
213
228
.
21.
Morrongiello
,
B. A.
,
Robson
,
R. C.
,
Best
,
C. T.
, and
Clifton
,
R. K.
(
1984
). “
Trading relations in the perception of speech by five-year-old children
,”
J. Exp. Child Psychol.
37
,
231
250
.
22.
Nittrouer
,
S.
(
1992
). “
Age-related differences in perceptual effects of formant transitions within syllables and across syllable boundaries
,”
J. Phonetics
20
,
1
32
.
23.
Nittrouer
,
S.
(
1996
). “
Discriminability and perceptual weighting of some acoustic cues to speech perception by 3-year-olds
,”
J. Speech Hear. Res.
39
,
278
297
.
24.
Nittrouer
,
S.
, and
Miller
,
M. E.
(
1997a
). “
Developmental weighting shifts for noise components of fricative-vowel syllables
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
102
,
572
580
.
25.
Nittrouer
,
S.
, and
Miller
,
M. E.
(
1997b
). “
Predicting developmental shifts in perceptual weighting schemes
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
101
,
2253
2266
.
26.
Nittrouer
,
S.
, and
Miller
,
M. E.
(
1999
). “
Developmental changes in perceptual weighting strategies for speech are contrast specific
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
106
,
S2246
.
27.
Nittrouer
,
S.
, and
Studdert-Kennedy
,
M.
(
1987
). “
The role of coarticulatory effects in the perception of fricatives by children and adults
,”
J. Speech Hear. Res.
30
,
319
329
.
28.
Nittrouer
,
S.
,
Crowther
,
C. S.
, and
Miller
,
M. E.
(
1998
). “
The relative weighting of acoustic properties in the perception of [s]+stop clusters by children and adults
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
60
,
51
64
.
29.
Nittrouer
,
S.
,
Miller
,
M. E.
,
Crowther
,
C. S.
, and
Manhart
,
M. J.
(
2000
). “
The effect of segmental order on fricative labeling by children and adults
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
62
,
266
284
.
30.
Parnell
,
M. M.
, and
Amerman
,
J. D.
(
1978
). “
Maturational influences on perception of coarticulatory effects
,”
J. Speech Hear Res.
21
,
682
701
.
31.
Remez
,
R. E.
,
Rubin
,
P. E.
,
Berns
,
S. M.
,
Pardo
,
J. S.
, and
Lang
,
J. M.
(
1994
). “
On the perceptual organization of speech
,”
Psychol. Rev.
101
,
129
156
.
32.
Stevens
,
K. N.
, and
Blumstein
,
S. E.
(
1978
). “
Invariant cues for place of articulation in stop consonants
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
64
,
1358
1368
.
33.
Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1983). “Limits on alternative auditory representations of speech,” in Cochlear Prostheses: An International Symposium, edited by C. W. Parkins and S. W. Anderson (The New York Academy of Sciences, New York), Vol. 405.
34.
Wardrip-Fruin
,
C.
, and
Peach
,
S.
(
1984
). “
Developmental aspects of the perception of acoustic cues in determining the voicing feature of final stop consonants
,”
Lang. Speech
27
,
367
379
.
35.
Whalen
,
D. H.
, and
Liberman
,
A. M.
(
1987
). “
Speech perception takes precedence over nonspeech perception
,”
Science
237
,
169
171
.
This content is only available via PDF.
You do not currently have access to this content.