Numerous studies of infants’ speech perception abilities have demonstrated that these young listeners have access to acoustic detail in the speech signal. Because these studies have used stimuli that could be described in terms of adult-defined phonetic categories, authors have concluded that infants innately recognize stimuli as members of these categories, as adults do. In fact, the predominant, current view of speech perception holds that infants are born with sensitivities for the universal set of phonetic boundaries, and that those boundaries supported by the ambient language are maintained, while those not supported by the ambient language dissolve. In this study, discrimination abilities of 46 infants and 75 3-year-olds were measured for several phonetic contrasts occurring in their native language, using natural and synthetic speech. The proportion of children who were able to discriminate any given contrast varied across contrasts, and no one contrast was discriminated by anything close to all of the children. While these results did not differ from those reported by others, the interpretation here is that we should reconsider the notion of innate phonetic categories and/or boundaries. Moreover, success rates did not differ for natural and synthetic speech, and so a minor conclusion was that children are not adversely affected by the use of synthetic stimuli in speech experiments.

1.
Aslin, R. N., and Pisoni, D. B. (1980). “Some developmental processes in speech perception,” in Child Phonology, edited by G. H. Yeni-Komshian, J. F. Kavanagh, and C. A. Ferguson (Academic, New York), Vol. 2, pp. 67–96.
2.
Aslin
,
R. N.
,
Pisoni
,
D. B.
,
Hennessy
,
B. L.
, and
Perey
,
A. J.
(
1981
). “
Discrimination of voice onset time by human infants: New findings and implications for the effects of early experience
,”
Child Dev.
52
,
1135
1145
.
3.
Bertoncini
,
J.
,
Bijeljac-Babic
,
R.
,
Jusczyk
,
P. W.
,
Kennedy
,
L. J.
, and
Mehler
,
J.
(
1988
). “
An investigation of young infants’ perceptual representations of speech sounds
,”
J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.
117
,
21
33
.
4.
Best, C. T. (1994). “The emergence of native-language phonological influences in infants: A perceptual assimilation model,” in The Development of Speech Perception, edited by J. C. Goodman and H. C. Nusbaum (MIT, Cambridge, MA), pp. 167–224.
5.
Dobie
,
R. A.
, and
Berlin
,
C. I.
(
1979
). “
Influence of otitis media on hearing and development,” in Otitis Media and Child Development: Speech, Language and Education
,”
Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol.
88
(Suppl. 60),
48
53
.
6.
Eilers
,
R. E.
,
Gavin
,
W.
, and
Oller
,
D. K.
(
1982
). “
Cross-linguistic perception in infancy: Early effects of linguistic experience
,”
J. Child Lang.
9
,
289
302
.
7.
Eilers
,
R. E.
,
Wilson
,
W. R.
, and
Moore
,
J. M.
(
1977
). “
Developmental changes in speech discrimination in infants
,”
J. Speech Hear. Res.
20
,
766
780
.
8.
Eimas
,
P. D.
(
1974
). “
Auditory and linguistic processing of cues for place of articulation by infants
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
16
,
513
521
.
9.
Eimas
,
P. D.
(
1975
). “
Auditory and phonetic coding of the cues for speech: Discrimination of the [r-l] distinction by young infants
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
18
,
341
347
.
10.
Eimas
,
P. D.
, and
Miller
,
J. L.
(
1980
). “
Discrimination of information for manner of articulation
,”
Infant Behav. Dev.
3
,
367
375
.
11.
Eimas, P. D., Miller, J. L., and Jusczyk, P. W. (1987). “On infant speech perception and the acquisition of language,” in Categorical Perception: The Groundwork of Cognition, edited by S. Harnard (Cambridge U. P., New York), pp. 161–195.
12.
Eimas
,
P. D.
,
Siqueland
,
E. R.
,
Jusczyk
,
P. W.
, and
Vigorito
,
J.
(
1971
). “
Speech perception in early infancy
,”
Science
171
,
304
306
.
13.
Greenlee
,
M.
(
1980
). “
Learning the phonetic cues to the voiced-voiceless distinction: A comparison of child and adult speech perception
,”
J. Child Lang.
7
,
459
468
.
14.
Grieser
,
D.
, and
Kuhl
,
P. K.
(
1989
). “
Categorization of speech by infants: Support for speech-sound prototypes
,”
Dev. Psychol.
25
,
577
588
.
15.
Hirsh-Pasek
,
K.
,
Kemler Nelson
,
D. G.
,
Jusczyk
,
P. W.
,
Wright Cassidy
,
K.
,
Druss
,
B.
, and
Kennedy
,
L.
(
1987
). “
Clauses are perceptual units for young infants
,”
Cognition
26
,
269
286
.
16.
Holmberg
,
T. L.
,
Morgan
,
K. A.
, and
Kuhl
,
P. K.
(
1977
). “
Speech perception in early infancy: Discrimination of fricative consonants
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
62
,
S99
.
17.
Jusczyk, P. W. (1995). “Language acquisition: Speech sounds and the beginning of phonology,” in Speech, Language, and Communication: Handbook of Perception and Cognition, Vol. 11, edited by J. L. Miller and P. D. Eimas (Academic, San Diego), pp. 263–301.
18.
Jusczyk, P. W. (1998). The Discovery of Spoken Language (MIT, Cambridge, MA).
19.
Jusczyk
,
P. W.
,
Pisoni
,
D. B.
, and
Mullennix
,
J.
(
1992
). “
Some consequences of stimulus variability on speech processing by two-month-old infants
,”
Cognition
43
,
253
291
.
20.
Klatt
,
D.
(
1980
). “
Software for a cascade/parallel formant synthesizer
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
67
,
971
995
.
21.
Krause
,
S. E.
(
1982
). “
Vowel duration as a perceptual cue to postvocalic consonant voicing in young children and adults
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
71
,
990
995
.
22.
Kuhl, P. K. (1979a). “The perception of speech in early infancy,” in Speech and Language: Advances in Basic Research and Practice, edited by N. J. Lass (Academic, New York), pp. 1–47.
23.
Kuhl
,
P. K.
(
1979b
). “
Speech perception in early infancy: Perceptual constancy for spectrally dissimilar vowel categories
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
66
,
1668
1679
.
24.
Kuhl, P. K. (1980). “Perceptual constancy for speech-sound categories in early infancy,” in Child Phonology, Vol. 2: Perception, edited by G. H. Yeni-Komshian, J. F. Kavanaugh, and C. A. Ferguson (Academic, New York), pp. 41–66.
25.
Kuhl
,
P. K.
(
1981
). “
Discrimination of speech by nonhuman animals: Basic auditory sensitivities conducive to the perception of speech sound catego-ries
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
70
,
340
349
.
26.
Kuhl, P. K. (1985). “Methods in the study of infant speech perception,” in Measurement of Audition and Vision in the First Year of Postnatal Life, edited by G. Gottlieb and N. A. Krasnegor (Ablex, Norwood, NJ), pp. 223–251.
27.
Kuhl, P. K. (1987). “Perception of speech and sound in early infancy,” in Handbook of Infant Perception, Vol. 2, edited by P. Salapatek and L. Cohen (Academic, New York), pp. 275–382.
28.
Kuhl
,
P. K.
(
1991
). “
Human adults and human infants show a ‘perceptual magnet effect’ for the prototypes of speech categories, monkeys do not
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
50
,
93
107
.
29.
Kuhl
,
P. K.
(
1993
). “
Developmental speech perception: Implications for models of language impairment
,”
Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci.
682
,
248
263
.
30.
Kuhl
,
P. K.
, and
Miller
,
J. D.
(
1978
). “
Speech perception by the chinchilla: Identification functions for synthetic VOT stimuli
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
63
,
905
917
.
31.
Kuhl
,
P. K.
, and
Miller
,
J. D.
(
1982
). “
Discrimination of auditory target dimensions in the presence or absence of variation in a second dimension by infants
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
31
,
279
292
.
32.
Lasky
,
R. E.
,
Syrdal-Lasky
,
A.
, and
Klein
,
R. E.
(
1975
). “
VOT discrimination by four- to six-and-a-half-month-old infants from Spanish environments
,”
J. Exp. Child. Psychol.
20
,
215
225
.
33.
Levitt
,
A. G.
,
Jusczyk
,
P. W.
,
Murray
,
J.
, and
Carden
,
G.
(
1988
). “
Context effects in two-month-old infants’ perception of labiodental/interdental fricative contrasts
,”
J. Exp. Psychol. [Hum Learn]
14
,
361
368
.
34.
Lisker
,
L.
, and
Abramson
,
A. S.
(
1964
). “
A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements
,”
Word
20
,
384
422
.
35.
Moffitt
,
A. R.
(
1971
). “
Consonant cue perception by twenty- to twenty-four-week-old infants
,”
Child Dev.
42
,
717
731
.
36.
Moore
,
J. M.
,
Thompson
,
G.
, and
Thompson
,
M.
(
1975
). “
Auditory localization in infants as a function of reinforcement conditions
,”
J. Speech Hear. Disord.
40
,
29
34
.
37.
Morrongiello
,
B. A.
,
Robson
,
R. C.
,
Best
,
C. T.
, and
Clifton
,
R. K.
(
1984
). “
Trading relations in the perception of speech by five-year-old children
,”
J. Exp. Child Psychol.
37
,
231
250
.
38.
Morse
,
P. A.
(
1972
). “
The discrimination of speech and nonspeech stimuli in early infancy
,”
J. Exp. Child Psychol.
14
,
477
492
.
39.
Morse, P. A. (1985). “Infant speech perception,” in Advances in the Study of Communication and Affect: Auditory Development in Children, Vol. 10, edited by S. E. Trehub and B. Schneider (Plenum, New York), pp. 215–230.
40.
Nittrouer
,
S.
(
1992
). “
Age-related differences in perceptual effects of formant transitions within syllables and across syllable boundaries
,”
J. Phonetics
20
,
1
32
.
41.
Nittrouer
,
S.
(
1996
). “
Discriminability and perceptual weighting of some acoustic cues to speech perception by 3-year-olds
,”
J. Speech Hear. Res.
39
,
278
297
.
42.
Nittrouer
,
S.
, and
Miller
,
M. E.
(
1997a
). “
Developmental weighting shifts for noise components of fricative-vowel syllables
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
102
,
572
580
.
43.
Nittrouer
,
S.
, and
Miller
,
M. E.
(
1997b
). “
Predicting developmental shifts in perceptual weighting schemes
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
101
,
2253
2266
.
44.
Nittrouer
,
S.
, and
Studdert-Kennedy
,
M.
(
1987
). “
The role of coarticulatory effects in the perception of fricatives by children and adults
,”
J. Speech Hear. Res.
30
,
319
329
.
45.
Nittrouer
,
S.
,
Crowther
,
C.
, and
Miller
,
M. E.
(
1998
). “
The relative weighting of acoustic properties in the perception of [s]+stop clusters by children and adults
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
60
,
51
64
.
46.
Nittrouer
,
S.
,
Miller
,
M. E.
,
Crowther
,
C. S.
, and
Manhart
,
M. J.
(
2000
). “
The effect of segmental order on fricative labeling by children and adults
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
62
,
266
284
.
47.
Nozza
,
R. J.
(
1987
). “
Infant speech-sound discrimination testing: Effects of stimulus intensity and procedural model on measures of performance
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
81
,
1928
1937
.
48.
Parnell
,
M. M.
, and
Amerman
,
J. D.
(
1978
). “
Maturational influences on perception of coarticulatory effects
,”
J. Speech Hear. Res.
21
,
682
701
.
49.
Sinex
,
D. G.
, and
McDonald
,
L. P.
(
1989
). “
Average discharge rate representation of voice onset time in the chinchilla auditory nerve
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
83
,
1817
1827
.
50.
Sinex
,
D. G.
,
McDonald
,
L. P.
, and
Mott
,
J. B.
(
1991
). “
Neural correlates of nonmonotonic temporal acuity for voice onset time
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
90
,
2441
2449
.
51.
Stevens, K. N. (1985). “Evidence for the role of acoustic boundaries in the perception of speech sounds,” in Phonetic Linguistics, edited by V. A. Fromkin (Academic, Orlando), pp. 243–255.
52.
Streeter
,
L.
(
1976
). “
Language perception shows effects of both innate mechanisms and experience
,”
Nature (London)
259
,
39
41
.
53.
Wardrip-Fruin
,
C.
, and
Peach
,
S.
(
1984
). “
Developmental aspects of the perception of acoustic cues in determining the voicing feature of final stop consonants
,”
Lang. Speech.
27
,
367
379
.
54.
Werker
,
J. F.
(
1989
). “
Becoming a native listener
,”
Am. Sci.
77
,
54
59
.
55.
Werker, J. (1991). “The ontogeny of speech perception,” in Modularity and the Motor Theory of Speech Perception, edited by I. G. Mattingly and M. Studdert-Kennedy (Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ), pp. 91–109.
56.
Werker, J. F. (1994). “Cross-language speech perception: Developmental change does not involve loss,” in The Development of Speech Perception, edited by J. C. Goodman and H. C. Nusbaum (MIT, Cambridge, MA), pp. 93–120.
57.
Werker
,
J. F.
, and
Tees
,
R. C.
(
1984
). “
Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life
,”
Infant Behav. Dev.
7
,
49
63
.
58.
Werker
,
J. F.
,
Gilbert
,
J. H. V.
,
Humphrey
,
K.
, and
Tees
,
R. C.
(
1981
). “
Developmental aspects of cross-language speech perception
,”
Child Dev.
52
,
349
355
.
This content is only available via PDF.
You do not currently have access to this content.