Two experiments examined the relationship between temporal pitch (and, more generally, rate) perception and auditory lateralization. Both used dichotic pulse trains that were filtered into the same high (3900–5400-Hz) frequency region in order to eliminate place-of-excitation cues. In experiment 1, a 1-s periodic pulse train of rate Fr was presented to one ear, and a pulse train of rate 2Fr was presented to the other. In the “synchronous” condition, every other pulse in the 2Fr train was simultaneous with a pulse in the opposite ear. In each trial, subjects concentrated on one of the two binaural images produced by this mixture: they matched its perceived location by adjusting the interaural level difference (ILD) of a bandpass noise, and its rate/pitch was then matched by adjusting the rate of a regular pulse train. The results showed that at low Fr (e.g., 2 Hz), subjects heard two pulse trains of rate Fr, one in the “higher rate” ear, and one in the middle of the head. At higher Fr (>25 Hz) subjects heard two pulse trains on opposite sides of the midline, with the image on the higher rate side having a higher pitch than that on the “lower rate” side. The results were compared to those in a control condition, in which the pulses in the two ears were asynchronous. This comparison revealed a duplex region at Fr>25 Hz, where across-ear synchrony still affected the perceived locations of the pulse trains, but did not affect their pitches. Experiment 2 used a 1.4-s 200-Hz dichotic pulse train, whose first 0.7 s contained a constant interaural time difference (ITD), after which the sign of the ITD alternated between subsequent pulses. Subjects matched the location and then the pitch of the “new” sound that started halfway through the pulse train. The matched location became more lateralized with increasing ITD, but subjects always matched a pitch near 200 Hz, even though the rate of pulses sharing the new ITD was only 100 Hz. It is concluded from both experiments that temporal pitch perception is not driven by the output of binaural mechanisms.

1.
Akerboom
,
S.
,
ten Hoopen
,
G.
,
Olierook
,
P.
, and
van der Schaaf
,
T.
(
1983
). “
Auditory spatial alternation transforms auditory time
,”
J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.
9
,
882
897
.
2.
Akeroyd, M. A., and Summerfield, A. Q. (1998). “Predictions of signal thresholds in a frozen-noise masker using monaural and binaural temporal windows,” in Psychophysical and Physiological Advances in Hearing, edited by A. R. Palmer, A. Rees, A. Q. Summerfield, and R. Meddis (Whurr, London), pp. 433–439.
3.
Akeroyd
,
M. A.
, and
Summerfield
,
A. Q.
(
1999
). “
A binaural analog of gap detection
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
105
,
2807
2820
.
4.
Axelrod
,
S.
,
Guzy
,
L. T.
, and
Diamond
,
I. T.
(
1967
). “
Perceived rate of monotic and dichotically alternating clicks
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
43
,
51
55
.
5.
Beerends
,
J. G.
, and
Houtsma
,
A. J. M.
(
1989
). “
Pitch identification of simultaneous diotic and dichotic two-tone complexes
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
85
,
813
819
.
6.
Blauert
,
J.
(
1972
). “
On the lag of lateralization caused by interaural time and intensity differences
,”
Audiology
11
,
265
270
.
7.
Buell
,
T. N.
,
Trahiotis
,
C.
, and
Bernstein
,
L. R.
(
1991
). “
Lateralization of low-frequency tones: Relative potency of gating and ongoing interaural delays
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
90
,
3077
3085
.
8.
Culling
,
J. F.
,
Summerfield
,
A. Q.
, and
Marshall
,
D. H.
(
1998a
). “
Dichotic pitches as illusions of binaural unmasking. Huggins’ pitch and the binaural edge pitch
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
103
,
3509
3526
.
9.
Culling
,
J. F.
,
Summerfield
,
A. Q.
, and
Marshall
,
D. H.
(
1998b
). “
Dichotic pitches as illusions of binaural unmasking. The Fourcin pitch and the dichotic repetition pitch
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
103
,
3527
3539
.
10.
Culling
,
J. F.
, and
Summerfield
,
Q.
(
1998
). “
Measurements of the binaural temporal window using a detection task
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
103
,
3540
3553
.
11.
Darwin, C. J., and Carlyon, R. P. (1995). “Auditory Grouping,” in Hearing, edited by B. C. J. Moore (Academic, Orlando), pp. 387–424.
12.
Darwin
,
C. J.
, and
Ciocca
,
V.
(
1992
). “
Grouping in pitch perception: Effects of onset asynchrony and ear of presentation of a mistuned component
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
91
,
3381
3390
.
13.
Darwin
,
C. J.
, and
Hukin
,
R. W.
(
1999
). “
Auditory objects of attention: The role of interaural time differences
,”
J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform.
25
,
617
629
.
14.
de Boer, E. (1956). “On the ‘Residue’ in Hearing,” Doctoral dissertation, Amsterdam.
15.
de Boer, E. (1976). “On the ‘Residue’ and Auditory Pitch Perception,” in Handbook of Sensory Physiology, edited by W. D. Keindel and W. D. Neff (Springer, Berlin), Vol. 3, pp. 479–584.
16.
Deutsch
,
D.
(
1974
). “
An auditory illusion
,”
Nature (London)
251
,
307
309
.
17.
Deutsch
,
D.
(
1976
). “
Separate ‘what’ and ‘where’ decision mechanisms in processing a dichotic tonal sequence
,”
J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform.
2
,
23
29
.
18.
Durlach, N. I. (1972). “Binaural signal detection: Equalization and cancellation theory,” in Foundations of Modern Auditory Theory, edited by J. V. Tobias (Academic, New York), Vol. II.
19.
Dye
,
R. H.
,
Stellmack
,
M. A.
,
Grange
,
A. N.
, and
Yost
,
W. A.
(
1996
). “
The effect of distractor frequency on judgments of target laterality based on interaural delays
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
99
,
1096
1107
.
20.
Efron
,
R.
, and
Yund
,
E. W.
(
1974
). “
Dichotic competition of simultaneous tones bursts of different frequency I. Dissociation of pitch from lateralization and loudness
,”
Neuropsychologia
12
,
249
256
.
21.
Franssen, N. V. (1960). “Some considerations of the mechanisms of directional hearing,” Ph.D. thesis, Delft.
22.
Freyman
,
R. L.
,
Zurek
,
P. M.
,
Balakrishnan
,
U.
, and
Chiang
,
Y.-C.
(
1997
). “
Onset dominance in lateralization
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
101
,
1649
1659
.
23.
Gockel
,
H.
, and
Carlyon
,
R. P.
(
1998
). “
Effects of ear of entry and perceived location of synchronous and asynchronous components on mistuning detection
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
104
,
3534
3545
.
24.
Gockel
,
H.
,
Carlyon
,
R. P.
, and
Micheyl
,
C.
(
1999
). “
Context dependence of fundamental frequency discrimination: Lateralized temporal fringes
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
106
,
3553
3563
.
25.
Godsmark
,
D.
, and
Brown
,
G. J.
(
1999
). “
A blackboard architecture for computational auditory scene analysis
,”
Speech Commun.
27
,
351
366
.
26.
Grantham
,
D. W.
(
1984
). “
Detectability of varying interaural intensity differences
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
76
,
71
76
.
27.
Grantham, D. W. (1995). “Spatial Hearing and Related Phenomena,” in Hearing, edited by B. C. J. Moore (Academic, Orlando), pp. 297–346.
28.
Grantham
,
D. W.
, and
Wightman
,
F. L.
(
1978
). “
Detectability of varying interaural temporal differences
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
63
,
511
523
.
29.
Guttman
,
N.
, and
Pruzansky
,
S.
(
1962
). “
Lower limits of pitch and musical pitch
,”
J. Speech Hear. Res.
5
,
207
214
.
30.
Hafter
,
E. F.
, and
Dye
,
R. H.
(
1983
). “
Detection of interaural differences of time in trains of high-frequency clicks as a function of interclick interval and number
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
73
,
644
651
.
31.
Hafter
,
E. F.
, and
Richards
,
V. M.
(
1988
). “
Discrimination of the rate of filtered impulses
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
43
,
405
414
.
32.
Hartmann
,
W. M.
, and
Johnson
,
D.
(
1991
). “
Stream segregation and peripheral channeling
,”
Music Percept.
9
,
155
184
.
33.
Heller
,
L. M.
, and
Trahiotis
,
C.
(
1996
). “
Extents of laterality and binaural interference effects
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
99
,
3632
3637
.
34.
Henning
,
G. B.
(
1974
). “
Detectability of interaural delay with high-frequency complex waveforms
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
55
,
84
90
.
35.
Houtsma, A. J. (1995). “Pitch Perception,” in Hearing, edited by B. C. J. Moore (Academic, Orlando), pp. 267–298.
36.
Huggins
,
A. W. F.
(
1974
). “
On perceptual integration of dichotically alternated pulse trains
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
56
,
939
943
.
37.
Kaernbach
,
C.
, and
Demany
,
L.
(
1998
). “
Psychophysical evidence against the autocorrelation theory of auditory temporal processing
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
104
,
2298
2306
.
38.
Koehnke
,
J.
,
Culotta
,
C. P.
,
Hawley
,
M. L.
, and
Colburn
,
H. S.
(
1995
). “
Effects of reference interaural time and intensity differences on binaural performance in listeners with normal and impaired hearing
,”
Ear Hear.
16
,
331
353
.
39.
Krumbholz
,
K.
,
Patterson
,
R. D.
, and
Pressnitzer
,
D.
(
2000
). “
The lower limit of pitch as determined by rate discrimination
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
108
,
1170
1180
.
40.
Kubovy
,
M.
(
1988
). “
Should we resist the seductiveness of the space:time∷vision:audition analogy?
J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform.
14
,
318
320
.
41.
Kuhn, G. F. (1987). “Physical acoustics and measurements pertaining to directional hearing,” in Directional Hearing, edited by W. A. Yost and G. Gourevitch (Springer, New York), pp. 3–25.
42.
Lyzenga
,
J.
, and
Carlyon
,
R. P.
(
2000
). “
Binaural effects in frequency modulation detection interference for vowel formants
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
108
,
753
759
.
43.
McFadden
,
D.
, and
Pasanen
,
E. G.
(
1976
). “
Lateralization at high frequencies based on interaural time differences
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
59
,
634
639
.
44.
McKay
,
C. M.
, and
Carlyon
,
R. P.
(
1999
). “
Dual temporal pitch percepts from acoustic and electric amplitude-modulated pulse trains
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
105
,
347
357
.
45.
Mossop
,
J. E.
, and
Culling
,
J. F.
(
1998
). “
Lateralization of large interaural delays
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
104
,
1574
1579
.
46.
Nakao
,
M. A.
, and
Axelrod
,
S.
(
1976
). “
Effects of bilateral alternation on perceived temporal uniformity of auditory and somasthetic pulse trains
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
20
,
274
280
.
47.
Plack
,
C. J.
, and
Carlyon
,
R. P.
(
1995
). “
Differences in frequency modulation detection and fundamental frequency discrimination between complex tones consisting of resolved and unresolved harmonics
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
98
,
1355
1364
.
48.
Pressnitzer
,
D.
,
Patterson
,
R. D.
, and
Krumbholz
,
K.
(
1999
). “
The lower limit of melodic pitch with filtered harmonic complexes
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
105
,
1152
.
49.
Ritsma
,
R. J.
(
1963
). “
On pitch discrimination of residue tones
,”
Int. Audiol.
2
,
34
37
.
50.
Rogers
,
W. L.
, and
Bregman
,
A. S.
(
1993
). “
An experimental evaluation of three theories of stream segregation
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
53
,
179
189
.
51.
Saberi
,
K.
(
1996
). “
Observer weighting of interaural delay in filtered impulses
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
58
,
1037
1046
.
52.
Shackleton
,
T. M.
, and
Carlyon
,
R. P.
(
1994
). “
The role of resolved and unresolved harmonics in pitch perception and frequency modulation discrimination
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
95
,
3529
3540
.
53.
Terhardt
,
E.
(
1977
). “
Pitch shift of monaural pure tones caused by contralateral sounds
,”
Acustica
37
,
56
57
.
54.
Thurlow
,
W. R.
(
1943
). “
Studies in auditory theory. I. Binaural interaction and the perception of pitch
,”
J. Exp. Psychol.
32
,
17
36
.
55.
Treisman
,
A.
(
1998
). “
Feature binding, attention, and object perception
,”
Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London, Ser. B
353
,
1265
1306
.
56.
Warren
,
R. M.
, and
Bashford
,
J. A.
(
1976
). “
Auditory contralateral induction: An early stage in binaural processing
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
20
,
380
386
.
57.
Wiegrebe
,
L.
, and
Patterson
,
R. D.
(
1999
). “
Quantifying the distortion products generated by SAM noise
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
106
,
2709
2718
.
58.
Wightman
,
F. L.
, and
Kistler
,
D. J.
(
1989
). “
The dominant role of low-frequency interaural time differences in sound localization
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
91
,
1648
1661
.
59.
Yost
,
W. A.
(
1981
). “
Lateralization position of sinusoids presented with interaural intensive and temporal differences
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
70
,
397
409
.
60.
Yost
,
W. A.
,
Wightman
,
F. L.
, and
Green
,
D. M.
(
1971
). “
Lateralization of filtered clicks
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
50
,
1526
1531
.
61.
Yund
,
E. W.
, and
Efron
,
R.
(
1975
). “
Dichotic competition of simultaneous tones bursts of different frequency—II. Suppression and ear dominance functions
,”
Neuropsychologia
13
,
137
150
.
62.
Zwicker
,
E.
,
Flottorp
,
G.
, and
Stevens
,
S. S.
(
1957
). “
Critical bandwidths in loudness summation
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
29
,
548
557
.
63.
Zwicker
,
T.
(
1984
). “
Experimente zur dichotischen Oktav-Tauschung
,”
Acustica
55
,
128
136
.
This content is only available via PDF.
You do not currently have access to this content.