Four experiments explored the relative contributions of spectral content and phonetic labeling in effects of context on vowel perception. Two 10-step series of CVC syllables ([bVb] and [dVd]) varying acoustically in F2 midpoint frequency and varying perceptually in vowel height from [ʌ] to [ɛ] were synthesized. In a forced-choice identification task, listeners more often labeled vowels as [ʌ] in [dVd] context than in [bVb] context. To examine whether spectral content predicts this effect, nonspeech–speech hybrid series were created by appending 70-ms sine-wave glides following the trajectory of CVC F2’s to 60-ms members of a steady-state vowel series varying in F2 frequency. In addition, a second hybrid series was created by appending constant-frequency sine-wave tones equivalent in frequency to CVC F2 onset/offset frequencies. Vowels flanked by frequencymodulated glides or steady-state tones modeling [dVd] were more often labeled as [ʌ] than were the same vowels surrounded by nonspeech modeling [bVb]. These results suggest that spectral content is important in understanding vowel context effects. A final experiment tested whether spectral content can modulate vowel perception when phonetic labeling remains intact. Voiceless consonants, with lower-amplitude more-diffuse spectra, were found to exert less of an influence on vowel perception than do their voiced counterparts. The data are discussed in terms of a general perceptual account of context effects in speech perception.

1.
Bailey, P. J., Summerfield, A. Q., and Dorman, M. (1977). “On the identification of sine wave analogues of certain speech sounds,” Haskins Lab. Status Report on Speech Res., SR-51/52, 1–25.
2.
von Bekesy, G. (1967). Sensory Inhibition (Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ).
3.
Best
,
C. T.
,
Morrongiello
,
B.
, and
Robson
,
R.
(
1981
). “
Perceptual equivalence of acoustic cues in speech and nonspeech perception
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
29
,
191
211
.
4.
Brady
,
P. T.
,
House
,
A. S.
, and
Stevens
,
K. N.
(
1961
). “
Perception of sounds characterized by a rapidly changing resonant frequency
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
33
,
1357
1362
.
5.
Brugge, J. F., and Reale, R. A. (1985). “Auditory cortex,” in Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 4, edited by A. Peters and E. G. Jones (Plenum, New York), pp. 229–271.
6.
Caspary
,
D. M.
,
Rupert
,
A. L.
, and
Moushegian
,
G.
(
1977
). “
Neuronal coding of vowel sounds in the cochlear nuclei
,”
Exp. Neurol.
54
,
414
431
.
7.
Dannenbring
,
G. L.
(
1976
). “
Perceived auditory continuity with alternately rising and falling frequency transitions
,”
Can. J. Psychol.
30
,
99
114
.
8.
Darwin
,
C. J.
(
1984
). “
Perceiving vowels in the presence of another sound: Constraints on formant perception
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
76
,
1636
1647
.
9.
Darwin
,
C. J.
,
Pattison
,
H.
, and
Gardner
,
R. B.
(
1989
). “
Vowel quality changes produced by surrounding tone sequences
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
45
,
333
342
.
10.
Darwin
,
C. J.
, and
Sutherland
,
N. S.
(
1984
). “
Grouping frequency components of vowels: When is a harmonic not a harmonic?
Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
36A
,
193
208
.
11.
Delgutte, B. (1996). “Auditory neural processing of speech,” in The Handbook of Phonetic Sciences, edited by W. J. Hardcastle and J. Laver (Blackwell, Oxford), pp. 507–538.
12.
Delgutte, B., Hammond, B. M., Kalluri, S., Litvak, L. M., and Carian, P. A. (1996). “Neural encoding of temporal envelope and temporal interactions in speech,” in Proceedings of Auditory Basis of Speech Perception, edited by W. Ainsworth and S. Greenberg, European Speech Communication Association.
13.
Diehl
,
R. L.
, and
Walsh
,
M. A.
(
1989
). “
Effects of syllable duration on stop-glide identification in syllable-initial and syllable-final position by humans and monkeys
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
85
,
2154
2164
.
14.
Fowler
,
C. A.
(
1986
). “
An event approach to the study of speech perception from a direct-realist perspective
,”
J. Phonetics
14
,
3
28
.
15.
Fowler
,
C. A.
(
1996
). “
Listeners do hear sounds, not tongues
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
99
,
1730
1741
.
16.
Fowler
,
C. A.
,
Best
,
C. T.
, and
McRoberts
,
G. W.
(
1990
). “
Young infants’ perception of liquid coarticulatory influences on following stop consonants
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
48
,
559
570
.
17.
Gibson
,
J. J.
(
1933
). “
Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of curved lines
,”
J. Exp. Psychol.
16
,
1
31
.
18.
Harris
,
D. M.
, and
Dallos
,
P.
(
1979
). “
Forward masking of auditory-nerve fiber responses
,”
J. Neurophysiol.
42
,
1083
1107
.
19.
Hartline
,
H. K.
, and
Ratliff
,
F.
(
1957
). “
Inhibitory interaction of receptor units in the eye of Limulus
,”
J. Gen. Physiol.
40
,
1357
1376
.
20.
Holt, L. L. (1999). “Auditory constraints on speech perception: An examination of spectral contrast,” unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Wisconsin–Madison.
21.
Holt
,
L. L.
, and
Kluender
,
K. R.
(in press). “General auditory processes contribute to perceptual accommodation of coarticulation,” Phonetica.
22.
Holt, L. L., and Rhode, W. R. (2000). “Examining context-dependent speech perception in the chinchilla cochlear nucleus,” Midwinter Meeting of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology.
23.
Irvine, D. R. F. (1992). “Physiology of the auditory brainstem,” in The Mammalian Auditory Pathway: Neurophysiology, edited by A. N. Popper and R. R. Fay (Springer Verlag, New York), pp. 153–231.
24.
Kiang, N. Y. S., Watanabe, T., Thomas, E. C., and Clark, L. F. (1965). “Discharge patterns of single fibers in the cat’s auditory nerve,” Res. Monogr., 35 (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA).
25.
Klatt
,
D. K.
(
1980
). “
Software for a cascade/parallel formant synthesizer
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
67
,
971
995
.
26.
Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt Psychology (Harcourt Brace, New York).
27.
Liberman
,
A. M.
,
Cooper
,
F. S.
,
Shankweiler
,
D. P.
, and
Studdert-Kennedy
,
M.
(
1957
). “
Perception of the speech code
,”
Psychol. Rev.
74
,
431
461
.
28.
Liberman
,
A. M.
, and
Mattingly
,
I. G.
(
1985
). “
The motor theory of speech perception revised
,”
Cognition
21
,
1
36
.
29.
Lindblom
,
B.
, and
Studdert-Kennedy
,
M.
(
1967
). “
On the role of formant transitions in vowel recognition
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
42
,
830
843
.
30.
Lisker
,
L.
, and
Abramson
,
A. S.
(
1964
). “
A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements
,”
Word
20
,
384
422
.
31.
Lisker
,
L.
, and
Abramson
,
A. S.
(
1971
). “
Distinctive features and laryngeal control
,”
Language
47
,
767
785
.
32.
Lotto, A. J. (1996). “General auditory constraints in speech perception,” unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Wisconsin–Madison.
33.
Lotto
,
A. J.
, and
Kluender
,
K. R.
(
1998
). “
General contrast effects of speech perception: Effect of preceding liquid on stop consonant identification
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
60
,
602
619
.
34.
Lotto
,
A. J.
,
Kluender
,
K. R.
, and
Green
,
K. P.
(
1996
). “
Spectral discontinuities and the vowel length effect
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
58
,
1005
1014
.
35.
Lotto
,
A. J.
,
Kluender
,
K. R.
, and
Holt
,
L. L.
(
1997
). “
Perceptual compensation for coarticulation by Japanese quail (Coturnix coturnix japonica)
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
102
,
1134
1140
.
36.
Mann
,
V. A.
(
1980
). “
Influence of preceding liquid on stop-consonant perception
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
28
,
407
412
.
37.
Mann
,
V. A.
(
1986
). “
Distinguishing universal and language-dependent levels of speech perception: Evidence from Japanese listeners’ perception of English “l” and “r”
,”
Cognition
24
,
169
196
.
38.
Mann
,
V. A.
, and
Repp
,
B. H.
(
1980
). “
Influence of vocalic context on perception of the [sh]–[s] distinction
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
28
,
213
228
.
39.
Mullennix, J. W., Pisoni, D. B., and Goldinger, S. D. (1988). “Some effects of time-varying context on the perception of speech and nonspeech sounds,” Res. on Speech Percept.: Prog. Report No. 14, Indiana University.
40.
Nábĕlek
,
I. V.
,
Nábĕlek
,
A. K.
, and
Hirsh
,
I. J.
(
1970
). “
Pitch of tone bursts of changing frequency
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
48
,
536
553
.
41.
Nábĕlek
,
I. V.
,
Nábĕlek
,
A. K.
, and
Hirsh
,
I. J.
(
1973
). “
Pitch of sound bursts with continuous or discontinuous change of frequency
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
53
,
1305
1312
.
42.
Nearey
,
T.
(
1989
). “
Static, dynamic, and relational properties in vowel perception
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
85
,
2088
2113
.
43.
Remez
,
R. E.
,
Rubin
,
P. E.
,
Pisoni
,
D. B.
, and
Carrell
,
T. D.
(
1981
). “
Speech perception without traditional speech cues
,”
Science
212
,
947
950
.
44.
Repp
,
B. H.
(
1982
). “
Phonetic trading relations and context effects: New experimental evidence for a speech mode of perception
,”
Psychol. Bull.
92
,
81
110
.
45.
Repp
,
B. H.
(
1983
). “
Bidirectional contrast effects in the perception of VC–CV sequences
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
33
,
147
155
.
46.
Ruggero, M. A. (1992). “Physiology and coding of sound in the auditory nerve,” in The Mammalian Auditory Pathway: Neurophysiology, edited by A. N. Popper and R. R. Fay (Springer Verlag, New York), pp. 34–93.
47.
Shigeno
,
S.
, and
Fugisaki
,
H.
(
1979
). “
Effect of a preceding anchor upon the categorical judgment of speech and nonspeech stimuli
,”
Japanese Psych. Res.
21
,
165
173
.
48.
Sinnott
,
J. M.
,
Brown
,
C. H.
, and
Borneman
,
M. A.
(
1998
). “
Effects of syllable duration on stop-glide identification in syllable-initial and syllable-final position by humans and monkeys
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
60
,
1032
1043
.
49.
Smith
,
R. L.
(
1979
). “
Adaptation, saturation, and physiological masking in single auditory-nerve fibers
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
65
,
166
178
.
50.
Stevens, K. N., and Halle, M. (1967). “Remarks on analysis by synthesis and distinctive features,” in Models for the Perception of Speech and Visual Form, edited by W. Wathem-Dunn (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA).
51.
Stevens
,
K. N.
, and
House
,
A. S.
(
1963
). “
Perturbations of vowel articulations by consonantal context: An acoustical study
,”
J. Speech Hear. Res.
6
,
111
128
.
52.
Summerfield
,
Q.
,
Haggard
,
M. P.
,
Foster
,
J.
, and
Gray
,
S.
(
1984
). “
Perceiving vowels from uniform spectra: Phonetic exploration of an auditory aftereffect
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
35
,
203
213
.
53.
Summerfield
,
Q.
,
Sidwell
,
A.
, and
Nelson
,
T.
(
1987
). “
Auditory enhancement of changes in spectral amplitude
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
81
,
700
707
.
54.
Warren
,
R. M.
(
1985
). “
Criterion shift rule and perceptual homeostasis
,”
Psychol. Rev.
92
,
574
584
.
55.
Williams, D. R. (1986). “Role of dynamic information in the perception of coarticulated vowels,” unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Connecticut.
This content is only available via PDF.
You do not currently have access to this content.