Normally, the apparent position of a sound source corresponds closely to its actual position. However, in some experimental situations listeners make large errors, such as indicating that a source in the frontal hemifield appears to be in the rear hemifield, or vice versa. These front–back confusions are thought to be a result of the inherent ambiguity of the primary interaural difference cues, interaural time difference (ITD) in particular. A given ITD could have been produced by a sound source anywhere on the so-called “cone of confusion.” More than 50 years ago Wallach [J. Exp. Psychol. 27, 339–368 (1940)] argued that small head movements could provide the information necessary to resolve the ambiguity. The direction of the change in ITD that accompanies a head rotation is an unambiguous indicator of the proper hemifield. The experiments reported here are a modern test of Wallach’s hypothesis. Listeners indicated the apparent positions of real and virtual sound sources in conditions in which head movements were either restricted or encouraged. The front–back confusions made in the restricted condition nearly disappeared in the condition in which head movements were encouraged. In a second experiment head movements were restricted, but the sound source was moved, either by the experimenter or by the listener. Only when the listener moved the sound source did front–back confusions disappear. The results clearly support Wallach’s hypothesis and suggest further that head movements are not required to produce the dynamic cues needed to resolve front–back ambiguity.

1.
Bronkhorst
,
A. W.
(
1995
). “
Localization of real and virtual sound sources
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
98
,
2542
2553
.
2.
Cleveland, W. S. (1985). The Elements of Graphing Data (Wadsworth, Monterey, CA), pp. 161–162.
3.
Durlach
,
N. I.
,
Rigopulos
,
A.
,
Pang
,
X. D.
,
Woods
,
W. S.
,
Kulkarni
,
A.
,
Colburn
,
H. S.
, and
Wenzel
,
E. M.
(
1992
). “
On the externalization of auditory images
,”
Presence
1
,
251
257
.
4.
Green, D. M. (1988). Profile Analysis (Oxford U.P., New York), pp. 93–96.
5.
Hartmann
,
W. M.
(
1983
). “
Localization of sound in rooms
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
74
,
1380
1391
.
6.
Kistler
,
D. J.
, and
Wightman
,
F. L.
(
1992
). “
A model of head-related transfer functions based on principal components analysis and minimum-phase reconstruction
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
91
,
1637
1647
.
7.
Makous
,
J. C.
, and
Middlebrooks
,
J. C.
(
1990
). “
Two-dimensional sound localization by human listeners
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
87
,
2188
2200
.
8.
Oldfield
,
S. R.
, and
Parker
,
S. P. A.
(
1984
). “
Acuity of sound localization: A topography of auditory space. I. Normal hearing conditions
,”
Percept.
13
,
581
600
.
9.
Perrett
,
S.
, and
Noble
,
W.
(
1997a
). “
The contribution of head motion cues to localization of low-pass noise
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
59
,
1018
1026
.
10.
Perrett
,
S.
, and
Noble
,
W.
(
1997b
). “
The effect of head rotations on vertical plane sound localization
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
102
,
2325
2332
.
11.
Pollack
,
I.
, and
Rose
,
M.
(
1967
). “
Effects of head movements on the localization of sounds in the equatorial plane
,”
Percept. Psychophys.
2
,
591
596
.
12.
Rakerd
,
B.
, and
Hartmann
,
W. M.
(
1985
). “
Localization of sound in rooms, II: The effects of a single reflecting surface
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
78
,
524
533
.
13.
Thurlow
,
W. R.
, and
Runge
,
P. S.
(
1967
). “
Effect of induced head movements on localization of direction of sounds
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
42
,
480
488
.
14.
Toole
,
F. W.
(
1970
). “
In-head localization of acoustic images
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
48
,
943
949
.
15.
Wallach
,
H.
(
1939
). “
On sound localization
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
10
,
270
274
.
16.
Wallach
,
H.
(
1940
). “
The role of head movements and vestibular and visual cues in sound localization
,”
J. Exp. Psychol.
27
,
339
368
.
17.
Wenzel
,
E. M.
(
1992
). “
Localization in virtual acoustic displays
,”
Presence
1
,
80
107
.
18.
Wenzel
,
E. M.
,
Arruda
,
M.
,
Kistler
,
D. J.
, and
Wightman
,
F. L.
(
1993
). “
Localization using nonindividualized head-related transfer functions
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
94
,
111
123
.
19.
Wightman
,
F. L.
, and
Kistler
,
D. J.
(
1989a
). “
Headphone simulation of free-field listening I: Stimulus synthesis
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
85
,
858
867
.
20.
Wightman
,
F. L.
, and
Kistler
,
D. J.
(
1989b
). “
Headphone simulation of free-field listening II: Psychophysical validation
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
85
,
868
878
.
21.
Wightman
,
F. L.
, and
Kistler
,
D. J.
(
1992
). “
The dominant role of low-frequency interaural time differences in sound localization
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
91
,
1648
1661
.
22.
Wightman, F. L., and Kistler, D. J. (1997a). “Factors affecting the relative salience of sound localization cues,” in Binaural and Spatial Hearing, edited by R. Gilkey and T. Anderson (Erlbaum, Mawah, NJ), pp. 1–23.
23.
Wightman
,
F. L.
, and
Kistler
,
D. J.
(
1997b
). “
Monaural sound localization revisited
,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
101
,
1050
1063
.
24.
Young
,
P. T.
(
1931
). “
The role of head movements in auditory localization
,”
J. Exp. Psychol.
14
,
95
124
.
This content is only available via PDF.
You do not currently have access to this content.