The usual answer to the question “Why is the sky blue?” is based only on Rayleigh scattering from the molecules in the atmosphere and makes little mention of the contribution of color vision. We supplement this answer with a quantitative discussion of the role color vision plays in determining the appearance of the daytime sky. The anatomy of the human eye is reviewed, and its response as a function of wavelength is described via the spectral sensitivities of the cones. Color matching is examined for a mixture of monochromatic lights and for the spectrum of the daytime sky. The spectral irradiance of skylight is shown to be a metameric match to unsaturated blue light. A simple experiment is described suitable for classroom use or a student project.

1.
R. A. Serway and J. W. Jewett, Jr., Physics for Engineers and Scientists with Modern Physics (Brooks/Cole Thomson, Belmont, CA, 2004), 6th ed., p. 1231.
2.
J. D. Jackson, Classical Electrodynamics (Wiley, New York, 1999), 3rd ed., pp. 465–470.
3.
M. P.
Thekaekara
,
R.
Kruger
, and
C. H.
Duncan
, “
Solar irradiance measurements from research aircraft
,”
Appl. Opt.
8
,
1713
1732
(
1969
).
4.
M. P.
Thekaekara
, “
Extraterrestrial solar spectrum, 3000–6000 Å at 1 Å intervals
,”
Appl. Opt.
13
,
518
522
(
1974
).
5.
M. P.
Thekaekara
, “
Solar energy outside the Earth’s atmosphere
,”
Sol. Energy
14
,
109
127
(
1973
).
6.
G. S. Smith, An Introduction to Classical Electromagnetic Radiation (Cambridge U. P., Cambridge, 1997), Sec. 7.7, pp. 521–537.
7.
The measurement for zenith skylight was made at Owings, MD, by Professor Raymond L. Lee, Jr., of the U. S. Naval Academy, Annapolis. It is for a field of view with the sun at an elevation of 33.2°. The measured result and that for Rayleigh scattering have been adjusted to the same value at the wavelength 550 nm.
8.
S. J. Williams and H. Z. Cummins, Light and Color in Nature and Art (Wiley, New York, 1983), p. 13.
9.
The role of the human eye in the determining the color of the sky is mentioned by
C. F.
Bohren
and
A. B.
Fraser
, “
Colors of the sky
,”
Phys. Teach.
23
,
267
272
(
1985
),
and in a number of research publications. See, for example,
J. V.
Dave
, “
Simulation colorimetry of the Earth-atmosphere system
,”
Remote Sens. Environ.
9
,
301
324
(
1980
).
10.
E. O.
Hulbert
, “
Explanation of the brightness and color of the sky, particularly the twilight sky
,”
J. Opt. Soc. Am.
43
,
113
118
(
1953
).
11.
G. Wyszecki and W. S. Stiles, Color Science: Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae (Wiley-Interscience, New York, 1982), 2nd ed.
12.
P. K. Kaiser and R. M. Boynton, Human Color Vision (Optical Society of America, Washington, DC, 1996), 2nd ed.
13.
K. R. Gegenfurtner and L. T. Sharpe, editors, Color Vision: From Genes to Perception (Cambridge U. P., Cambridge, 1999).
14.
S. K. Shevell, editor, The Science of Color (Optical Society of America, Washington, DC, 2003), 2nd ed.
15.
T. Lamb and J. Bourriau, editors, Colour: Art and Science (Cambridge U. P., Cambridge, 1995).
16.
P. L. Pease, Color and Color Vision: Selected Reprints (AAPT, Stony Brook, NY, 1982).
17.
I. Newton, Opticks (London, 1704); Republication of 4th ed. from 1730 (Dover, New York, 1952), pp. 124–125.
18.
C. S.
MacLatchy
, “
The diameter of the fovea – a short exercise for the life-sciences
,”
Am. J. Phys.
46
,
1076
1077
(
1978
).
19.
R. M.
Boynton
, “
History and current status of a physiologically based system of photometry and colorimetry
,”
J. Opt. Soc. Am.
13
,
1609
1621
(
1996
).
20.
J. K.
Bowmaker
and
H. J. A.
Dartnall
, “
Visual pigments of rods and cones in human retina
,”
J. Physiol. (London)
298
,
501
511
(
1980
).
21.
D. A.
Baylor
,
B. J.
Nunn
, and
J. L.
Schnapf
, “
Spectral sensitivity of cones of the monkey Macaca fascicularis
,”
J. Physiol. (London)
390
,
145
160
(
1987
).
22.
J. L.
Schnapf
,
T. W.
Kraft
, and
D. A.
Baylor
, “
Spectral sensitivity of human cone photoreceptors
,”
Nature (London)
325
,
439
441
(
1987
).
23.
J. L.
Schnapf
and
D. A.
Baylor
, “
How photoreceptor cells respond to light
,”
Sci. Am.
256
,
40
47
(
1987
).
24.
A. Stockman and L. T. Sharpe, “Cone spectral sensitivities and color matching,” Color Vision: From Genes to Perception, edited by K. R. Gegenfurtner and L. T. Sharpe (Cambridge U. P., Cambridge, 1999), Chap. 2.
25.
A.
Stockman
,
L. T.
Sharpe
, and
C.
Fach
, “
The spectral sensitivities of human short-wavelength sensitive cones derived from thresholds and color matches
,”
Vision Res.
39
,
2901
2927
(
1999
).
26.
A.
Stockman
and
L. T.
Sharpe
, “
The spectral sensitivities of the middle- and long-wavelength-sensitive cones derived from measurements in observers of known genotype
,”
Vision Res.
40
,
1711
1737
(
2000
).
27.
The cone spectral sensitivities of Stockman and Sharpe are also available in tabular form at 〈http://cvrl.ioo.ucl.ac.uk〉.
28.
T.
Young
, “
On the theory of light and colours
,”
Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London
92
,
12
48
(
1802
).
29.
A. Wood, Thomas Young: Natural Philosopher 1773–1829 (Cambridge U. P., Cambridge, 1954), pp. 104–115.
30.
J. D. Mollon, “The origins of modern color science,” The Science of Color, edited by S. K. Shevell (Optical Society of America, Washington, DC, 2003), 2nd ed., Chap. 1.
31.
In the actual experiment, the two beams in Fig. 4(b) would completely overlap and be adjusted in amplitude to produce a spot indistinguishable from the yellow one in Fig. 4(a). This procedure avoids the possibility that the adjacent colors (red and green) will change the appearance of the yellow region.
32.
Lord
Rayleigh
, “
Experiments on colour
,”
Nature (London)
25
,
64
66
(
1881
).
33.
J. N.
Howard
, “
John William Strutt, third Baron Rayleigh
,”
Appl. Opt.
3
,
1091
1101
(
1964
).
34.
J. D. Morelend, “Developments in anomaloscopy,” in John Dalton’s Colour Vision Legacy, edited by C. Dickinson, I. Murray, and D. Carden (Taylor and Francis, London, 1997), Chap. 6.1.
35.
The lamps required modifications to make their performance acceptable. Spaces around the filter holders had to be covered to prevent the leakage of unfiltered light. The holders for the bulb, filter, and lens had to be adjusted to align the axes for these three elements. Before this alignment, the illuminated disk produced by superimposing the beams from two lamps was not uniform in color. The bulbs in the three lamps were replaced with the model described in the text.
36.
When the reflector of one of these bulbs is viewed with the power off, it has a blue hue.
37.
It is important to realize that we are only adjusting the irradiance of the monochromatic blue light relative to that of the white light to match the ratio of irradiances for skylight. We cannot adjust the overall irradiance of the mixture to equal that of skylight, because the maximum irradiance for the white light is fixed by the output of the bulb.
38.
C. F. Bohren, “Optics, Atmosphere,” in Encyclopedia of Applied Physics, edited by G. L. Trigg (VCH, New York, 1995), Vol. 12, pp. 405–434.
39.
K. L. Coulson, Polarization and Intensity of Light in the Atmosphere (Deepak, Hampton, VA, 1988).
40.
L. T. Sharpe, A. Stockman, H. Jagle, and J. Nathans, “Opsin genes, cone photopigments, color vision, and color blindness,” in Color Vision: From Genes to Perception, edited by K. R. Gegenfurtner and L. T. Sharpe (Cambridge U. P., Cambridge, 1999), Chap. 1.
This content is only available via PDF.
AAPT members receive access to the American Journal of Physics and The Physics Teacher as a member benefit. To learn more about this member benefit and becoming an AAPT member, visit the Joining AAPT page.